Click consonant

Click consonants, or clicks, are speech sounds that occur as consonants in many languages of Southern Africa and in three languages of East Africa. Examples familiar to English-speakers are the Tut-tut (British spelling) or Tsk! Tsk! (American spelling) used to express disapproval or pity, the tchick! used to spur on a horse, and the clip-clop! sound children make with their tongue to imitate a horse trotting.

Airstreams
See also

Technically, clicks are obstruents articulated with two closures (points of contact) in the mouth, one forward and one at the back. The enclosed pocket of air is rarefied by a sucking action of the tongue (in technical terminology, clicks have a lingual ingressive airstream mechanism). The forward closure is then released,[note 1] producing what may be the loudest consonants in the language, but in some languages such as Hadza and Sandawe, clicks can be more subtle and may even be mistaken for ejectives.

Notation and sound description

ǀ ǁ ǂ ǃ ʘ
Click consonants
In UnicodeU+01C0 ǀ LATIN LETTER DENTAL CLICK

U+01C1 ǁ LATIN LETTER LATERAL CLICK
U+01C2 ǂ LATIN LETTER ALVEOLAR CLICK
U+01C3 ǃ LATIN LETTER RETROFLEX CLICK

U+0298 ʘ LATIN LETTER BILABIAL CLICK
Different from
Different fromU+007C | VERTICAL LINE
U+2016 DOUBLE VERTICAL LINE

Click consonants occur at six principal places of articulation. The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) may represent a click by placing the assigned symbol for five of the places of articulation (there is as yet no dedicated symbol for the sixth) adjacent to a symbol for a non-click sound at the rear place of articulation, or the click symbol may be combined with diacritics for voicing, nasalization, etc. The IPA symbols are used in writing most Khoisan languages, but Bantu languages such as Zulu typically use Latin c, x and q for dental, lateral and alveolar clicks respectively.

  • The easiest clicks for English speakers are the dental clicks written with a single pipe, ǀ. They are all sharp (high-pitched) squeaky sounds made by sucking on the front teeth. A simple dental click is used in English to express pity or to shame someone, and sometimes to call an animal, and is written tut! in British English and tsk! in American English. In Italian this sound means "no" used as an answer to a direct question, while it is used to call cats when repeated several times.
  • Next most familiar to English speakers are the lateral clicks written with a double pipe, ǁ. They are also squeaky sounds, though less sharp than ǀ, made by sucking on the molars on either side (or both sides) of the mouth. A simple lateral click is made in English to get a horse moving, and is conventionally written tchick!
  • Then there are the labial clicks, written with a bull's eye, ʘ. These are lip-smacking sounds, but without the pursing of the lips found in a kiss.

The above clicks sound like affricates, in that they involve a lot of friction. The other two families are more abrupt sounds that do not have this friction.

  • With the alveolar clicks, written with an exclamation mark, ǃ, the tip of the tongue is pulled down abruptly and forcefully from the roof of the mouth, sometimes using a lot of jaw motion, and making a hollow pop! like a cork being pulled from an empty bottle. These sounds can be quite loud.
  • Finally, for most languages, the palatal clicks, ǂ, are made with a flat tongue, and are sharper popping sounds than the ǃ clicks, like sharply snapped fingers.

Languages with clicks

Southern Africa

Clicks occur in all three Khoisan language families of southern Africa, where they may be the most numerous consonants. To a lesser extent they occur in three neighbouring groups of Bantu languages—which borrowed them, directly or indirectly, from Khoisan. In the southeast, in eastern South Africa, Swaziland, Lesotho, Zimbabwe and southern Mozambique, they were adopted from a Tuu language or languages by the languages of the Nguni cluster (especially Zulu, Xhosa and Phuthi, but also to a lesser extent Swazi and Ndebele), and spread from them in a reduced fashion to the Zulu-based pidgin Fanagalo, Sesotho, Tsonga, Ronga, the Mzimba dialect of Tumbuka and more recently to Ndau and urban varieties of Pedi, where the spread of clicks continues. The second point of transfer was near the Caprivi Strip and the Okavango River where, apparently, the Yeyi language borrowed the clicks from a West Kalahari Khoe language; a separate development led to a smaller click inventory in the neighbouring Mbukushu, Kwangali, Gciriku, Kuhane and Fwe languages in Angola, Namibia, Botswana and Zambia.[1] These sounds occur not only in borrowed vocabulary, but have spread to native Bantu words as well, in the case of Nguni at least partially due to a type of word taboo called hlonipha. Some creolised varieties of Afrikaans, such as Oorlams, retain clicks in Khoekhoe words.

East Africa

Three languages in East Africa use clicks: Sandawe and Hadza of Tanzania, and Dahalo, an endangered South Cushitic language of Kenya that has clicks in only a few dozen words. It is thought the latter may remain from an episode of language shift.

Damin

The only non-African language known to have clicks as regular speech sounds is Damin, a ritual code once used by speakers of Lardil in Australia. One of the clicks in Damin is actually an egressive click, using the tongue to compress the air in the mouth for an outward (egressive) "spurt".

Spread of clicks from loanwords

Once clicks are borrowed into a language as regular speech sounds, they may spread to native words, as has happened due to hlonipa word-taboo in the Nguni languages. In Gciriku, for example, the European loanword tomate (tomato) appears as cumáte with a click c, though it begins with a t in all neighbouring languages.

Marginal usage of clicks

Scattered clicks are found in ideophones and mimesis in other languages, such as Kongo /ᵑǃ/, Mijikenda /ᵑǀ/ and Hadza /ᵑʘʷ/ (Hadza does not otherwise have labial clicks). Ideophones often use phonemic distinctions not found in normal vocabulary.

English and many other languages may use bare clicks in interjections, without the accompaniment of vowels, such as the dental "tsk-tsk" sound used to express disapproval, or the lateral tchick used with horses. In Armenian, Bulgarian, Greek, Levantine Arabic, Maltese, Persian, Romanian, Turkish, occasionally in French, as well as southern Italian languages such as Sicilian, in which a bare dental click accompanied by tipping the head upwards signifies "no". Libyan Arabic apparently has three such sounds.

Clicks occasionally turn up elsewhere, as in the special registers twins sometimes develop with each other. In West Africa, clicks have been reported allophonically, and similarly in French and German, faint clicks have been recorded in rapid speech where consonants such as /t/ and /k/ overlap between words.[2] In Rwanda, the sequence /mŋ/ may be pronounced either with an epenthetic vowel, [mᵊŋ], or with a light bilabial click, [mʘ̃ŋ]—often by the same speaker.

Speakers of Gan Chinese from Ningdu county, as well as speakers of Mandarin from Beijing and Jilin and presumably people from other parts of the country, produce flapped nasal clicks in nursery rhymes with varying degrees of competence, in the words for 'goose' and 'duck', both of which begin with /ŋ/ in Gan and until recently began with /ŋ/ in Mandarin as well. In Gan, the nursery rhyme is,

tʰien i tsʰak ᵑǃ¡o 'a goose in the sky'
ti ha i tsʰak ᵑǃ¡a 'a duck on the ground'
ᵑǃ¡o saŋ ᵑǃ¡o tʰan, ᵑǃ¡o pʰau ᵑǃ¡o 'a goose lays a goose egg, a goose hatches a goose'
ᵑǃ¡a saŋ ᵑǃ¡a tʰan, ᵑǃ¡a pʰau ᵑǃ¡a 'a duck lays a duck egg, a duck hatches a duck'

where the /ŋ/ onsets are all pronounced [ᵑǃ¡].[3]

Occasionally other languages are claimed to have click sounds in general vocabulary. This is usually a misnomer for ejective consonants, which are found across much of the world.

Position in word

For the most part, the Southern African Khoisan languages only use root-initial clicks.[note 2] Hadza, Sandawe and several Bantu languages also allow syllable-initial clicks within roots. In no language does a click close a syllable or end a word, but then the languages that happen to have clicks have mostly CV syllables and allow at most only a limited set of consonants (such as a nasal or a glottal stop) to close a syllable or end a word, most consonants share the distribution of clicks in these languages.

Types of clicks used

The Khoesan families (Tuu, Kxʼa and Khoe) all have at least four click types, {ǀ ǁ ǃ ǂ} or variants thereof. A few have five, with bilabial } or retroflex {‼}. Hadza and Sandawe have three, {ǀ ǁ ǃ}. Yeyi is the only Bantu language with four, {ǀ ǁ ǃ ǂ}, while Xhosa and Zulu have three, {ǀ ǁ ǃ}, and most Bantu languages with clicks have fewer than that.

Types of clicks

Like other consonants, clicks can be described using four parameters: place of articulation, manner of articulation, phonation (including glottalisation) and airstream mechanism. As noted above, clicks necessarily involve at least two closures, which in some cases operate partially independently: an anterior articulation traditionally represented by the special click symbol in the IPA—and a posterior articulation traditionally described as oral or nasal, voiced or voiceless, etc. The literature also describes a contrast between velar and uvular rear articulations for some languages.

However, recent work shows that in languages that make this distinction, all clicks have a uvular, or even pharyngeal, rear closure—and the clicks explicitly described as uvular are in fact clusters/contours of a click plus a pulmonic or ejective component, in which the cluster/contour has two release bursts, the forward (click) and then the rearward (uvular) component. "Velar" clicks in these languages have only a single release burst, that of the forward click release, and the release of the rear articulation isn't separately audible (Miller 2011).

Nonetheless, in most of the literature the stated place of the click is the anterior articulation (called the release or influx), whereas the manner is ascribed to the posterior articulation (called the accompaniment or efflux). The anterior articulation defines the click type and is written with the IPA letter for the click (dental ǀ, alveolar ǃ, etc.), whereas the traditional term 'accompaniment' conflates the categories of manner (nasal, affricated), phonation (voiced, aspirated, breathy voiced, glottalised), as well as any change in the airstream with the release of the posterior articulation (pulmonic, ejective), all of which are transcribed with additional letters or diacritics, as in the nasal alveolar click, ǃŋ or ᵑǃ or—to take an extreme example—the voiced (uvular) ejective alveolar click, ᶢǃ͡qʼ.

The size of click inventories ranges from as few as three (in Sesotho) or four (in Dahalo), to dozens in the Kxʼa and Tuu (Northern and Southern Khoisan) languages. Taa, the last vibrant language in the latter family, has 45 to 115 click phonemes, depending on analysis (clusters vs. contours), and over 70% of words in the dictionary of this language begin with a click.[4]

Clicks appear more stop-like (sharp/abrupt) or affricate-like (noisy) depending on their place of articulation: In southern Africa, clicks involving an apical alveolar or laminal postalveolar closure are acoustically abrupt and sharp, like stops, whereas labial, dental and lateral clicks typically have longer and acoustically noisier click types that are superficially more like affricates. In East Africa, however, the alveolar clicks tend to be flapped, whereas the lateral clicks tend to be more sharp.

Transcription

The five click places of articulation with dedicated symbols in the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) are labial ʘ, dental ǀ, palatal ("palato-alveolar") ǂ, (post)alveolar ("retroflex") ǃ and lateral ǁ. In most languages, the alveolar and palatal types are abrupt; that is, they are sharp popping sounds with little frication (turbulent airflow). The labial, dental and lateral types, on the other hand, are typically noisy: they are longer, lip- or tooth-sucking sounds with turbulent airflow, and are sometimes called affricates. (This applies to the forward articulation; both may also have either an affricate or non-affricate rear articulation as well.) The apical places, ǃ and ǁ, are sometimes called "grave", because their pitch is dominated by low frequencies; whereas the laminal places, ǀ and ǂ, are sometimes called "acute", because they are dominated by high frequencies. (At least in the Nǁng language and Juǀʼhoan, this is associated with a difference in the placement of the rear articulation: "grave" clicks are uvular, whereas "acute" clicks are pharyngeal.) Thus the alveolar click /ǃ/ sounds something like a cork pulled from a bottle (a low-pitch pop), at least in Xhosa; whereas the dental click /ǀ/ is like English tsk! tsk!, a high-pitched sucking on the incisors. The lateral clicks are pronounced by sucking on the molars of one or both sides. The labial click /ʘ/ is different from what many people associate with a kiss: the lips are pressed more-or-less flat together, as they are for a [p] or an [m], not rounded as they are for a [w].

The most populous languages with clicks, Zulu and Xhosa, use the letters c, q, x, by themselves and in digraphs, to write click consonants. Most Khoisan languages, on the other hand (with the notable exceptions of Naro and Sandawe), use a more iconic system based on the pipe |. (The exclamation point for the "retroflex" click was originally a pipe with a subscript dot, along the lines of ṭ, ḍ, ṇ used to transcribe the retroflex consonants of India.) There are also two main conventions for the second letter of the digraph as well: voicing may be written with g and uvular affrication with x, or voicing with d and affrication with g (a convention of Afrikaans). In two orthographies of Juǀʼhoan, for example, voiced /ᶢǃ/ is written g! or dq, and /ᵏǃ͡χ/ !x or qg. In languages without /ᵏǃ͡χ/, such as Zulu, /ᶢǃ/ may be written gq.

Competing orthographies
labiallaminalapicalsubapical
dentalpalatalalveolarlateralretroflex
Lepsius (1855) ǀǀ́ ǀ̣ ǀǀ
Doke (1926) ɋʇʗʖψ
Beach (1938) ʘʇʄʗʖ
Bantu pccv ç tc
qc
qx
Khoikhoi ʘǀǂǃǁ
  1. ^ was proposed by Clement Doke,[5] and ʄ by Beach,[6] but did not catch on, and are not supported by Unicode. (Doke's character resembles a down arrow and is here represented by the old Roman numeral for 50;[note 3] Beach is a double-barred esh.) Three of these, ʇ, ʗ and ʖ, were adopted into the IPA, though eventually abandoned. Doke and Beach used additional or modified letters for voiced and nasal clicks, but they did not catch on.
  2. ^ The labial and palatal clicks do not occur in written Bantu languages. However, the palatal clicks have been romanised in Naron, Juǀʼhõasi and !Xun, where they have been written tc, ç and qc, respectively. In the 19th century, they were sometimes written v, which might be source of the Doke letter .

There are a few less-well-attested articulations. A reported subapical retroflex articulation in Grootfontein !Kung[note 4] turns out to be alveolar with lateral release, ǃǁ; Ekoka !Kung has a fricated alveolar click with an s-like release, provisionally transcribed ǃ͡s; and Sandawe has a "slapped" alveolar click, provisionally transcribed ǃ¡ (in turn, the lateral clicks in Sandawe are more abrupt and less noisy than in southern Africa). However, the Khoisan languages are poorly attested, and it is quite possible that, as they become better described, more click articulations will be found.

Formerly when a click consonant was transcribed, two symbols were used, one for each articulation, and connected with a tie bar. This is because a click such as [ŋ͡ǂ] was analysed as a nasal velar rear articulation [ŋ] pronounced simultaneously with the forward ingressive release [ǂ]. The symbols may be written in either order, depending on the analysis: ŋ͡ǂ or ǂ͡ŋ. However, a tie bar was not often used in practice, and when the manner is tenuis (a simple [k]), it was often omitted as well. That is, ǂ = = ǂk = k͡ǂ = ǂ͡k. Regardless, elements that do not overlap with the forward release are usually written according to their temporal order: Prenasalisation is always written first (ŋɡ͡ǂ = ŋǂ͡ɡ = ŋǂ̬), and the non-lingual part of a contour is always written second (k͡ǂʼqʼ = ǂ͡kʼqʼ = ǂ͡qʼ).

However, it has become standard to analyse clicks as simplex segments, despite the fact that the front and rear articulations are independent, and to use diacritics to indicate the rear articulation and the accompaniment. At first this tended to be ᵏǂ, ᶢǂ, ᵑǂ for k͡ǂ, ɡ͡ǂ, ŋ͡ǂ, based on the belief that the rear articulation was velar; but as it has become clear that the rear articulation is often uvular or even pharyngeal even when there is no velar–uvular contrast, voicing and nasalisation diacritics more in keeping with the IPA have started to appear: ǂ̥, ǂ̬, ǂ̃, ŋǂ̬ for ᵏǂ, ᶢǂ, ᵑǂ, ŋᶢǂ.

Evolution of accompaniment transcription
TenuisAspiratedVoicedNasalised"Uvular"
Coarticulation
analysis
k͡ǂ ~ ǂ͡k
(kǂ ~ ǂk)
k͡ǂʰ ~ ǂ͡kʰ
(kǂʰ ~ ǂkʰ)
ɡ͡ǂ ~ ǂ͡ɡ
(ɡǂ ~ ǂɡ)
ŋ͡ǂ ~ ǂ͡ŋ
(ŋǂ ~ ǂŋ)
q͡ǂ ~ ǂ͡q
(qǂ ~ ǂq)
Unified, velar ᵏǂᵏǂʰᶢǂᵑǂqǂ
IPA diacritics ǂ̥ǂʰǂ̬ǂ̃ǂ͡q

In practical orthography, the voicing or nasalisation is sometimes given the anterior place of articulation: dc for ᶢǀ and for ᵑʘ, for example.

In the literature on Damin, the clicks are transcribed as m!, nh!, n!, rn!.

Places of articulation

Places of articulation are often called click types, releases, or influxes, though 'release' is also used for the accompaniment/efflux. There are seven or eight known places of articulation, not counting slapped or egressive clicks. These are (bi)labial affricated ʘ, or "bilabial"; laminal denti-alveolar affricated ǀ, or "dental"; apical (post)alveolar plosive ǃ, or "alveolar"; laminal palatal plosive ǂ, or "palatal"; laminal palatal affricated ǂᶴ (known only from Ekoka !Kung); subapical postalveolar , or "retroflex" (only known from Central !Kung and possibly Damin); and apical (post)alveolar lateral ǁ, or "lateral".

Basic oral clicks
LabialDentalAlveolarRetroflexDomedPalatalLateralVelar
ʘ̥ ʘ̬ǀ̥ ǀ̬ǃ̥ ǃ̬‼̥ ‼̬ǂ̥ᶴ ǂ̬ᶴǂ̥ ǂ̬ǁ̥ ǁ̬NA

Languages illustrating each of these articulations are listed below. Given the poor state of documentation of Khoisan languages, it is quite possible that additional places of articulation will turn up. No language is known to contrast more than five.

Click place
inventory
LanguagesNotes
dental ǀ onlyDahaloVarious nasal clicks only.
1 release, variable ǀ ~ ǃ ~ ǁSotho, Gciriku, MbukushuIn Sotho the clicks tend to be alveolar, in Gciriku and Mbukushu dental.
2 releases, ǀ, ǂKwadiǂ not found with all manners
2 releases, ǀ, ǁǁXegwiǃ reacquired in loans
3 releases, ǀ, ǃ, ǁSandawe, Hadza, Xhosa, ZuluIn Hadza and Sandawe, ǃ is often "slapped".
4 releases, ǀ, ǂ, ǃ, ǁKorana, Khoekhoe, Yeyi, Juǀʼhoan
4 releases, ǀ, ǂᶴ, ǃ, ǁEkoka !Kung
5 releases, ʘ, ǀ, ǂ, ǃ, ǁǂHõã, Nǀu, ǀXam, Taa
5 releases, ǀ, ǂ, ǃ, , ǁGrootfontein !Kung
5 releases, ʘ, ʘ↑, ǀ, ǃ, DaminAside from /ʘ↑/, all are nasal.

Extra-linguistically, Coatlán Zapotec of Mexico uses a linguolabial click, [ǀ̼ʔ], as mimesis for a pig drinking water,[7] and several languages, such as Wolof, use a velar click [ʞ], long judged to be physically impossible, for backchanneling and to express approval.[8] A sublingual click ("sucking-teeth") is found across West Africa, the Caribbean and into the United States.

The exact place of the alveolar clicks varies between languages. The lateral, for example, is alveolar in Khoekhoe but postalveolar or even palatal in Sandawe; the central is alveolar in Nǀuu but postalveolar in Juǀʼhoan.[9]

Names found in the literature

The terms for the click types were originally developed by Bleek in 1862.[10] Since then there has been some conflicting variation. However, apart from "cerebral" (retroflex), which was found to be an inaccurate label when true retroflex clicks were discovered, Bleek's terms are still considered normative today. Here are the terms used in some of the main references.

Names in the literature
Click typeBleek (1862)Doke (1926)IPA (1928)Beach (1938)IPA (1949)IPA (1989)UnicodeMiller et al. (2009)[11]Vossen (2013)[12]other
ǀdentaldentaldentaldental affricativedentaldentaldentaldenti-pharyngealdentalalveolar affricated; denti-alveolar; apico-lamino-dental
ǃcerebralpalato-alveolarcerebralalveolar implosiveretroflex(post-)alveolarretroflexcentral alveo-uvularalveolarpalatal; palatal retroflex; apico-palatal
ǁlaterallateralalveolar laterallateral affricativelateral(alveolar) laterallaterallateral alveo-uvularlateral-alveolarpost-alveolar lateral; lateral apico-alveo-palatal
ǂpalatalalveolarvelardenti-alveolar implosivevelarpalatoalveolaralveolarpalato-pharyngealpalatalalveolar instantaneous; dental
ʘbilabialbilabiallabio-uvularbilabiallabial

The dental, lateral and bilabial clicks are rarely confused. However, the palatal and alveolar clicks frequently had conflicting names in older literature, and still do in Unicode. However, since Ladefoged & Traill (1984) clarified the places of articulation, the terms listed under Vosser (2013) have become standard, apart from details such as whether ǃ and ǁ are alveolar and postalveolar, or ʘ is bilabial or labiodental, in a particular language, or whether the rear articulation is velar, uvular or pharyngeal, which again varies between languages (or may even be contrastive within a language).

The back-vowel constraint

The shape of the tongue in Nama when articulating an alveolar click (blue) and a palatal click (red) [throat to the right]. The articulation of the vowel [i] is slightly forward of the red line, with its peak coinciding with the dip of the blue line.

In several languages, including Nama and Juǀʼhoan, the alveolar click types [ǃ] and [ǁ] only occur, or preferentially occur, before back vowels, whereas the dental and palatal clicks occur before any vowel. The effect is most noticeable with the high front vowel [i]. In Nama, for example, the diphthong [əi] is common but [i] is rare after alveolar clicks, whereas the opposite is true after dental and palatal clicks. This is a common effect of uvular or uvularised consonants on vowels in both click and non-click languages. In Taa, for example, the back-vowel constraint is triggered by both alveolar clicks and uvular stops, but not by palatal clicks or velar stops: sequences such as */ǃi/ and */qi/ are rare to non-existent, whereas sequences such as /ǂi/ and /ki/ are common. It is also triggered by labial clicks, though not by labial stops. Clicks subject to this constraint involve a sharp retraction of the tongue during release.

Abrupt
release
Noisy
release
ballistic tongue retraction
& back-vowel constraint
ǃǁ, ʘ
no retraction, no constraint ǂǀ

Miller and colleagues (2003) used ultrasound imaging to show that the rear articulation of the alveolar clicks ([ǃ]) in Nama is substantially different from that of palatal and dental clicks. Specifically, the shape of the body of the tongue in palatal clicks is very similar to that of the vowel [i], and involves the same tongue muscles, so that sequences such as [ǂi] involved a simple and quick transition. The rear articulation of the alveolar clicks, however, is several centimetres further back, and involves a different set of muscles in the uvular region. The part of the tongue required to approach the palate for the vowel [i] is deeply retracted in [ǃ], as it lies at the bottom of the air pocket used to create the vacuum required for click airstream. This makes the transition required for [ǃi] much more complex and the timing more difficult than the shallower and more forward tongue position of the palatal clicks. Consequently, [ǃi] takes 50 ms longer to pronounce than [ǂi], the same amount of time required to pronounce [ǃəi].

Languages do not all behave alike. In Nǀuu, the simple clicks /ʘ, ǃ, ǁ/ trigger the [əi] and [æ] allophones of /i/ and /e/, whereas /ǀ, ǂ/ do not. All of the affricated contour clicks, such as /ǂ͡χ/, do as well, as do the uvular stops /q, χ/. However, the occlusive contour clicks pattern like the simple clicks, and /ǂ͡q/ does not trigger the back-vowel constraint. This is because they involve tongue-root raising rather than tongue-root retraction in the uvular-pharyngeal region. However, in Gǀwi, which is otherwise largely similar, both /ǂ͡q/ and /ǂ͡χ/ trigger the back-vowel constraint (Miller 2009).

Manners of articulation

Click manners are often called click accompaniments or effluxes, but both terms have met with objections on theoretical grounds.

There is a great variety of click manners, both simplex and complex, the latter variously analysed as consonant clusters or contours. With so few click languages, and so little study of them, it is also unclear to what extent clicks in different languages are equivalent. For example, the [ǃkˀ] of Khoekhoe, [ǃkˀ ~ ŋˀǃk] of Sandawe and [ŋ̊ǃˀ ~ ŋǃkˀ] of Hadza may be essentially the same phone; no language distinguishes them, and the differences in transcription may have more to do with the approach of the linguist than with actual differences in the sounds. Such suspected allophones/allographs are listed on a common row in the table below.

Some Khoisan languages are typologically unusual in allowing mixed voicing in non-click consonant clusters/contours, such as dt͡sʼk͡xʼ, so it is not surprising that they would allow mixed voicing in clicks as well. This may be an effect of epiglottalised voiced consonants, because voicing is incompatible with epiglottalisation.

Phonation

As do other consonants, clicks vary in phonation. Oral clicks are attested with four phonations: tenuis, aspirated, voiced and breathy voiced (murmured). Nasal clicks may also vary, with plain voiced, breathy voiced / murmured nasal, aspirated and unaspirated voiceless clicks attested (the last only in Taa). The aspirated nasal clicks are often said to have 'delayed aspiration'; there is nasal airflow throughout the click, which may become voiced between vowels, though the aspiration itself is voiceless. A few languages also have pre-glottalised nasal clicks, which have very brief prenasalisation but have not been phonetically analysed to the extent that other types of clicks have.

All languages have nasal clicks, and all but Dahalo and Damin also have oral clicks. All languages but Damin have at least one phonation contrast as well.

Complex clicks

Clicks may be pronounced with a third place of articulation, glottal. A glottal stop is made during the hold of the click; the (necessarily voiceless) click is released, and then the glottal hold is released into the vowel. Glottalised clicks are very common, and they are generally nasalised as well. The nasalisation cannot be heard during the click release, as there is no pulmonic airflow, and generally not at all when the click occurs at the beginning of an utterance, but it has the effect of nasalising preceding vowels, to the extent that the glottalised clicks of Sandawe and Hadza are often described as prenasalised when in medial position. Two languages, Gǀwi and Yeyi, contrast plain and nasal glottalised clicks, but in languages without such a contrast, the glottalised click is nasal. Miller (2011) analyses the glottalisation as phonation, and so considers these to be simple clicks.

Various languages also have prenasalised clicks, which may be analysed as consonant sequences. Sotho, for example, allows a syllabic nasal before its three clicks, as in nnqane 'the other side' (prenasalised nasal) and seqhenqha 'hunk'.

There is ongoing discussion as to how the distinction between what were historically described as 'velar' and 'uvular' clicks is best described. The 'uvular' clicks are only found in some languages, and have an extended pronunciation that suggests that they are more complex than the simple ('velar') clicks, which are found in all. Nakagawa (1996) describes the extended clicks in Gǀwi as consonant clusters, sequences equivalent to English st or pl, whereas Miller (2011) analyses similar sounds in several languages as click–non-click contours, where a click transitions into a pulmonic or ejective articulation within a single segment, analogous to how English ch and j transition from occlusive to fricative but still behave as unitary sounds. With ejective clicks, for example, Miller finds that although the ejective release follows the click release, it is the rear closure of the click that is ejective, not an independently articulated consonant. That is, in a simple click, the release of the rear articulation is not audible, whereas in a contour click, the rear (uvular) articulation is audibly released after the front (click) articulation, resulting in a double release.

These contour clicks may be linguo-pulmonic, that is, they may transition from a click (lingual) articulation to a normal pulmonic consonant like [q] (e.g. [ǂ͡q]); or linguo-glottalic and transition from lingual to an ejective consonant like [] (e.g. [ǂ͡qʼ]): that is, a sequence of ingressive (lingual) release + egressive (pulmonic or glottalic) release. In some cases there is a shift in place of articulation as well, and instead of a uvular release, the uvular click transitions to a velar or epigottal release (depending on the description, [ǂ͡kxʼ] or [ǂᴴ]). Although homorganic [ǂ͡χʼ] does not contrast with heterorganic [ǂ͡kxʼ] in any known language, they are phonetically quite distinct (Miller 2011).

Apart from Dahalo, Damin and many of the Bantu languages (Yeyi and Xhosa being exceptions), 'click' languages have glottalised clicks. Contour clicks are restricted to southern Africa, but are very common there: they are found in all members of the Tuu, Kxʼa and Khoe families, as well as in the Bantu language Yeyi.

Variation among languages

In a comparative study of clicks across various languages, using her own field work as well as phonetic descriptions and data by other field researchers, Miller (2011) posits 21 types of clicks that contrast in manner or airstream.[note 5] The homorganic and heterorganic affricated ejective clicks do not contrast in any known language, but are judged dissimilar enough to keep separate. Miller's conclusions differ from those of the primary researcher of a language; see the individual languages for details.

(all spoken primarily in South Africa, Namibia and Botswana; Khoekhoe is like Korana except it has lost ejective /ᵏꞰ͡χʼ/)

(Zulu is like Xhosa apart from not having /ᵑꞰˀ/)

Each language below is illustrated with Ʞ as a placeholder for the different click types. Under each language are the orthography (in italics, with old forms in parentheses), the researchers' transcription (in angle brackets), or allophonic variation (in [brackets]). Some languages also have labialised or prenasalised clicks as well as those listed below.

               LanguageTuuKxʼaKhoeSandaweHadzaCushiticBantuAustralian
TaaNǁngǂʼAmkoeJuǀʼhoan[note 6]KoranaGǀuiDahaloXhosaYeyiDamin
Manner                ʘ, ǂ, ǃ, ǁ, ǀǂ, ǃ, ǁ, ǀǃ, ǁ, ǀǀǃ, ǁ, ǀǂ, ǃ, ǁ, ǀʘ, ‼, ǃ, ǀ
Simple
oral
click
Tenuis/ᵏꞰ/ * [ᵏꞰ] Ʞ (c, ç, q, x) Ʞg kꞰ c, q, x c, q, x (Ʞ) c, q, x
Voiced/ᶢꞰ/ gꞰ* ᶢꞰ [ᶢꞰ] gꞰ (dq etc.) gꞰ gq etc.
[ᶢꞰ ~ ŋᶢꞰ]
gꞰ
Aspirated/ᵏꞰʰ/ Ʞh* Ʞʰ [ᵏꞰʰ] Ʞh (qh etc.) Ʞkh kꞰh qh etc. qh etc. (Ʞh) qh etc. Ʞh (= Ʞx ?)
Breathy-voiced/ᶢꞰʱ/ gꞰh* gꞰh (dqh etc.)
[ᶢꞰʱ ~ ᶢꞰˠ]
gq etc.[note 7]
Simple
nasal
click
Voiceless/ᵑ̊Ʞ/ nhꞰ*
[ŋ̊ᵑꞰ]
Voiced/ᵑꞰ/ nꞰ*
[ŋ̈ᵑꞰ]
ᵑꞰ [ᵑꞰ] nꞰ (nq etc.) Ʞn ŋꞰ nq etc. nq etc. (nꞰ) /ᵑǀ/ nq etc. ŋꞰ
(Delayed) aspiration
(prenasalised between vowels)
/ᵑ̊Ʞʰʱ/ Ʞhh
[ŋ̊↓Ʞh]
ᵑ̊Ʞʰ [ᵑ̊Ʞʱ ~ ŋᵑ̊Ʞʱ] Ʞʼh (qʼh etc.) Ʞh ŋꞰh
Breathy-voiced/ᵑꞰʱ/ nꞰhh nꞰh (nqh etc.) ngq etc.[note 8]
Preglottalised nasal click/ˀᵑꞰ/ ʼnꞰ* [ʔᵑꞰ](in Ekoka)
Glottalised
click
Oral / velar ejective/ᵏꞰʼ/ Ʞʼ* kꞰʼ Ʞʼ
Creaky-voiced oral/ᶢꞰʼ/ gꞰʼ*
Nasal (silent initially,
prenasalised after vowels)
/ᵑ̊Ʞˀ/ Ʞʼʼ ᵑ̊Ʞˀ [Ʞˀ ~ ŋˀꞰ] Ʞʼ (qʼ etc.)
(w/ nasal vowels)
kꞰʔ (ŋ̊Ʞʔ) qʼ etc.
[Ʞˀʔ ~ ŋʔꞰˀ]
qq etc.
(Ʞʼ ~ nꞰʼ)
/ᵑǀˀ/ nkq etc. ?[14] ŋꞰʼ
Nasal (prenasalised initially) /ᵑꞰˀ/ nꞰʼʼ
Pulmonic
contour
Tenuis stop/Ʞ͡q/ Ʞq Ʞq [Ʞq] qꞰ
Voiced (and prenasalised)/ᶢꞰ͡ɢ/ gꞰq
[ᶰꞰɢ ~ Ʞɢ]
[Ʞɢ][note 9] ([ᶰꞰɢ])[note 10] ɢꞰ
[ᶰꞰɢ]
Aspirated stop/Ʞ͡qʰ/ Ʞqh Ʞqʰ [Ʞqʰ] qꞰh
Breathy-voiced /ᶢꞰ͡ɢʱ/ gꞰqh
Voiceless fricative /ᵏꞰ͡χ/ Ʞx Ʞχ [Ʞq͡χ] Ʞx (qg etc.) qꞰχ Ʞx (?)
Voiced fricative (prenasalised) /ᶢꞰ͡ʁ/ gꞰx
[ᶢꞰ͡χ ~ ɴᶢꞰ͡ʁ]
gꞰx (dqg etc.)
Ejective
contour
Ejective stop/Ʞ͡qʼ/ Ʞqʼ [Ʞqʼ] [Ʞqʼ] qꞰʼ
Voiced ejective stop/ᶢꞰ͡qʼ/ gꞰqʼ
Ejective fricative/Ʞ͡χʼ/ Ʞχʼ [Ʞq͡χʼ] Ʞkhʼ Ʞqʼ
Heterorganic affricate /
epiglottalised
/Ʞ͡kxʼ/ Ʞqxʼ Ʞk (qgʼ etc.)
[Ʞᵸ]
qꞰχʼ
Voiced heterorganic
affricate / epiglottalised
/ᶢꞰ͡kxʼ/ gꞰqxʼ gꞰk (dqgʼ etc.)
[ᶢꞰˤ]
Egressive[note 11](Voiceless "spurt"; labial only)/ʘ↑/
IPATaaNǁngǂʼAmkoeJuǀʼhoanKoranaGǀuiSandaweHadzaDahaloXhosaYeyiDamin

Yeyi also has prenasalised /ŋᶢꞰ/. The original researchers believe that [Ʞʰ] and [Ʞχ] are allophones.

A DoBeS (2008) study of the Western ǃXoo dialect of Taa found several new manners: creaky voiced (the voiced equivalent of glottalised oral), breathy-voiced nasal, prenasalised glottalised (the voiced equivalent of glottalised) and a (pre)voiced ejective. These extra voiced clicks reflect Western ǃXoo morphology, where many nouns form their plural by voicing their initial consonant. DoBeS analyses most Taa clicks as clusters, leaving nine basic manners (marked with asterisks in the table). This comes close to Miller's distinction between simple and contour clicks, shaded light and medium grey in the table.

Phonotactics

Languages of the southern African Khoisan families only permit clicks at the beginning of a word root. However, they also restrict other classes of consonant, such as ejectives and affricates, to root-initial position. The Bantu languages, Hadza and Sandawe allow clicks within roots.

In some languages, all click consonants within known roots are the same phoneme, as in Hadza cikiringcingca /ǀikiɺiN.ǀiN.ǀa/ 'pinkie finger', which has three tenuis dental clicks. Other languages are known to have the occasional root with different clicks, as in Xhosa ugqwanxa /uᶢ̊ǃʱʷaᵑǁa/ 'black ironwood', which has a slack-voiced alveolar click and a nasal lateral click.

Like other articulatorily complex consonants, clicks tend to be found in lexical words rather than in grammatical words, but this is only a tendency. In Nǁng, for example, there are two sets of personal pronouns, a full one without clicks and a partial set with clicks (ńg 'I', á 'thou', í 'we all', ú 'you', vs. nǀǹg 'I', gǀà 'thou', gǀì 'we all', gǀù 'you'), as well as other grammatical words with clicks such as ǁu 'not' and nǀa 'with, and'.

Click genesis and click loss

One genetic study concluded that clicks, which occur in the languages of the genetically divergent populations Hadza and !Kung, may be an ancient element of human language.[15] However, this conclusion relies on several dubious assumptions (see Hadza language), and most linguists assume that clicks, being quite complex consonants, arose relatively late in human history. How they arose is not known, but it is generally assumed that they developed from sequences of non-click consonants, as they are found allophonically for doubly articulated consonants in West Africa,[16] where /tk/ sequences overlap at word boundaries in German,[2] and for the sequence /mw/ in Ndau and Tonga.[note 12] Such developments have also been posited in historical reconstruction. For example, the Sandawe word for 'horn', /tɬana/, with a lateral affricate, may be a cognate with the root /ᵑǁaː/ found throughout the Khoe family, which has a lateral click. This and other words suggests that at least some Khoe clicks may have formed from consonant clusters when the first vowel of a word was lost; in this instance *[tɬana] > *[tɬna] > [ǁŋa] ~ [ᵑǁa].

On the other side of the equation, several non-endangered languages in vigorous use demonstrate click loss. For example, the East Kalahari languages have lost clicks from a large percentage of their vocabulary, presumably due to Bantu influence. As a rule, a click is replaced by a consonant with close to the manner of articulation of the click and the place of articulation of the forward release: alveolar click releases (the [ǃ] family) tend to mutate into a velar stop or affricate, such as [k], [ɡ], [ŋ], [k͡x]; palatal clicks ([ǂ] etc.) tend to mutate into a palatal stop such as [c], [ɟ], [ɲ], [cʼ], or a post-alveolar affricate [tʃ], [dʒ]; and dental clicks ([ǀ] etc.) tend to mutate into an alveolar affricate [ts].

Difficulty

Clicks are often presented as difficult sounds to articulate within words. However, children acquire them readily; a two-year-old, for example, may be able to pronounce a word with a lateral click [ǁ] with no problem, but still be unable to pronounce [s].[17] Lucy Lloyd reported that after long contact with the Khoi and San, it was difficult for her to refrain from using clicks when speaking English.[18]

See also

Notes

  1. This is the case for all clicks that constitute consonants in words. Paralinguistically, however, there are other methods of making clicks: under the tongue and by releasing the rear occlusion first. See #Places of articulation.
  2. Exceptions occurs in words borrowed from Bantu languages, which may have click in the middle.
  3. ɋ, ʇ, ʗ have descenders; , ʖ have ascenders.
  4. (a triple pipe) in Cole (1966) may have been the same thing. The Doke letter resembled ψ, or more precisely an inverted (descender only).
  5. Not counting the egressive "spurt" in Damin, and tree additional voiced manners in Western ǃXoo, which pair up with voiceless manners.
  6. Ekoka ǃKung has an additional manner, ˀᵑꞰ. Grootfontein and Mangetti Dune ǃKung, on the other hand, have a substantially smaller inventory: ᵏꞰ, ᶢꞰ, Ʞʰ, ᵑꞰ, ᵑ̊Ʞʱ, ᵑꞰˀ, Ʞ͡χ, Ʞ͡kxʼ.
  7. Perhaps better described as slack voice. Tone-depressor effect.[13]
  8. Tone-depressor effect. Sometimes a prenasalized click with a short, voiced oral occlusion, but usually without.
  9. not prenasalized
  10. perhaps borrowed from Gǀui
  11. Not technically a click, but the only other attested sound with a lingual airstream mechanism.
  12. Here the labial [m] may have assimilated to the velar place of the [w], as [m͡ŋw], with the release of the labial before the velar later generating a click [ᵐʘw]

References

  1. Derek Nurse & Gérard Philippson (2003) The Bantu languages, pp 31–32
  2. Fuchs, Sussanne; Koenig, Laura; Winkler, Ralf (2007). Weak clicks in German (PDF). Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Saarbrücken. pp. 449–452.
  3. Geoffrey Nathan, 'Clicks in a Chinese Nursery Rhyme', JIPA (2001) 31/2.
  4. L&M 1996, p 246
  5. Clement M Doke, 1926 (1969), The phonetics of the Zulu language. Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand Press
  6. Douglas Martyn Beach, 1938, The phonetics of the Hottantot language. W. Heffer & sons. ltd.
  7. Rosemary Beam de Azcona, Sound Symbolism. Available at http://www.linguistics.berkeley.edu/~rosemary/55-fall2003-onomatopoeia.pdf Archived 23 June 2007 at the Wayback Machine
  8. Lenore Grenoble (2014) "Verbal gestures: Toward a field-based approach to language description". In Plungian et al. (eds.), Language. Constants. Variables: In memory of A. E. Kibrik, 105–118. Aleteija: Saint Petersburg.
  9. Amanda Miller (2011) "The Representation of Clicks", The Blackwell Companion to Phonology.
  10. Wilhelm Bleek (1862) A Comparative Grammar of South African Languages, vol. 1, pp. 12–13
  11. Miller, Brugman, Sands, Namaseb, Exter & Collins (2009) Differences in airstream and posterior place of articulation among Nǀuu clicks, Journal of the International Phonetic Association 39, 129–161.
  12. Rainer Vosser (2013) Introduction, The Khoesan Languages, Routledge
  13. Jessen & Roux, 2002. Voice quality differences associated with stops and clicks in Xhosa
  14. According to Nurse & Philippson (2003:616). This is typically transcribed as a prenasalized click, and is not included in Miller.
  15. Tishkoff, S. A.; Gonder, M. K.; Henn, B. M.; et al. (2007). "History of click-speaking populations of Africa inferred from mtDNA and Y chromosome genetic variation". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 24 (10): 2180–95. doi:10.1093/molbev/msm155. PMID 17656633.
  16. Ladefoged 1968.
  17. Kirk Miller, 'Highlights of Hadza fieldwork'. LSA, San Francisco, 2009.
  18. Beach (1938), p 269.

Bibliography

  • Ladefoged, Peter (1968). A phonetic study of West African languages: An auditory-instrumental survey (2nd ed.). ISBN 0-521-06963-7.CS1 maint: ref=harv (link)
  • Ladefoged, Peter; Maddieson, Ian (1996). The Sounds of the World's Languages. Oxford: Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-19815-4.
  • Amanda Miller, Levi Namaseb, Khalil Iskarous. 2003. Tongue Body constriction differences in click types.
  • Amanda Miller, 2011. "The Representation of Clicks". In Oostendorp et al. eds., The Blackwell Companion to Phonology.
  • Traill, Anthony & Rainer Vossen. 1997. Sound change in the Khoisan languages: new data on click loss and click replacement. J African Languages and Linguistics 18:21–56.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.