Lunar effect

The lunar effect is a correlation between specific stages of the roughly 29.5-day lunar cycle and behavior and physiological changes in living beings on Earth, including humans. In some cases the purported effect may depend on external cues, such as the amount of moonlight. In other cases, such as the approximately monthly cycle of menstruation in humans (but not other mammals), the coincidence in timing reflects no known lunar influence.

Biologists as well as artists and poets have long thought about the Moon's influence on living creatures.
A lunar syzygy increases tidal range importantly for some maritime species. Despite all of the beliefs, no valid scientific study has ever found another significant effect of the full Moon on life on Earth.

A considerable number of studies have examined the effect on humans. By the late 1980s, there were at least 40 published studies on the purported lunar-lunacy connection,[1] and at least 20 published studies on the purported lunar-birthrate connection.[2] This has allowed several extensive literature reviews and meta-analyses to be produced, which have found no correlation between the lunar cycle and human biology or behavior.[1][2][3][4]

Origins of the belief

Examples of the belief have been found in ancient Assyrian/Babylonian writing.[5]

The Late Latin word lunaticus had a secondary meaning of “moonstruck”.

Contexts

Claims of a lunar connection have appeared in the following contexts:

Fertility

It is widely believed that the Moon has a relationship with fertility due to the corresponding human menstrual cycle, which averages 28 days.[5][6][7] However, no connection between lunar rhythms and menstrual onset has been conclusively shown to exist, and the similarity in length between the two cycles is most likely coincidental.[8][9]

Birth rate

Three studies carried out between 1959 and 1973 reported a 1 percent increase in births in New York following a full Moon. However, multiple studies have found no connection between birth rate and lunar phases. A 1957 analysis of 9,551 births in Danville, Pennsylvania, found no correlation between birth rate and the phase of the Moon.[10] Records of 11,961 live births and 8,142 natural births (not induced by drugs or cesarean section) over a 4-year period (1974–1978) at the UCLA hospital did not correlate in any way with the cycle of lunar phases.[11] Analysis of 3,706 spontaneous births (excluding births resulting from induced labor) in 1994 showed no correlation with lunar phase.[12] The distribution of 167,956 spontaneous vaginal deliveries, at 37 to 40 weeks gestation, in Phoenix, Arizona, between 1995 and 2000, showed no relationship with lunar phase.[13] Analysis of 564,039 births (1997 to 2001) in North Carolina showed no predictable influence of the lunar cycle on deliveries or complications.[14] Analysis of 6,725 deliveries (2000 to 2006) in Hannover revealed no significant correlation of birth rate to lunar phases.[15] A 2001 analysis of 70,000,000 birth records from the National Center for Health Statistics revealed no correlation between birth rate and lunar phase.[16] An extensive review of 21 studies from seven different countries showed that the majority of studies reported no relationship to lunar phase, and that the positive studies were inconsistent with each other.[2] A review of six additional studies from five different countries similarly showed no evidence of relationship between birth rate and lunar phase.[17]

Blood loss

It is sometimes claimed that surgeons used to refuse to operate during the full Moon because of the increased risk of death of the patient through blood loss.[18] One team, in Barcelona, Spain, reported a weak correlation between lunar phase and hospital admissions due to gastrointestinal bleeding, but only when comparing full Moon days to all non-full Moon days lumped together.[18] This methodology has been criticized, and the statistical significance of the results disappears if one compares day 29 of the lunar cycle (full Moon) to days 9, 12, 13, or 27 of the lunar cycle, which all have an almost equal number of hospital admissions.[19] The Spanish team acknowledged that the wide variation in the number of admissions throughout the lunar cycle limited the interpretation of the results.[18]

In October 2009, British politician David Tredinnick asserted that during a full Moon "[s]urgeons will not operate because blood clotting is not effective and the police have to put more people on the street.".[20] A spokesman for the Royal College of Surgeons said they would "laugh their heads off" at the suggestion they could not operate on the full Moon.[21]

Human behavior

Mental Illness

Two studies found evidence that those with mental disorders i.e. Schizophrenia generally exhibit 1.8% (< 2.0 percent is statistically irrelevant) of increased violent or aggressive episodes during the full Moon,[22][23] but a more recent study found no such correlation to that of nonschizophrenic human beings.[24] An analysis of mental-health data found a significant effect of Moon phases, but only on schizophrenic patients.[25] Such effects are not necessarily related directly to the appearance of the Moon.

Epilepsy

A study into epilepsy found a significant negative correlation between the mean number of seizures per day and the fraction of the Moon that is illuminated, but this correlation disappeared when the local clarity of the night sky was controlled for, suggesting that it is the brightness of the night sky, and not the lunar phase per se, that influences the occurrence of epileptic seizures with advanced photosensitive epilepsy.[26]

Law and order

Senior police officers in Brighton, UK, announced in June 2007 that they were planning to deploy more officers over the summer to counter trouble they believe is linked to the lunar cycle.[27] This followed research by the Sussex Police force that concluded there was a rise in violent crime when the Moon was full. A spokeswoman for the police force said "research carried out by us has shown a correlation between violent incidents and full moons". A police officer responsible for the research told the BBC that "From my experience of 19 years of being a police officer, undoubtedly on full moons we do seem to get people with sort of strange behavior – more fractious, argumentative."[28]

Police in Ohio and Kentucky have blamed temporary rises in crime on the full Moon.[29][30][31] In January 2008, New Zealand's Justice Minister Annette King suggested that a spate of stabbings in the country could have been caused by the lunar cycle.[32]

A reported correlation between Moon phase and the number of homicides in Dade County was found, through later analysis, not to be supported by the data and to have been the result of inappropriate and misleading statistical procedures.[3]

Motorcycle fatalities

A study of 13,029 motorcyclists killed in nighttime crashes found that there were 5.3% more fatalities on nights with a full moon compared to other nights.[33] The authors speculate that the increase might be due to visual distractions created by the moon, especially when it is near the horizon and appears abruptly between trees, around turns, etc.

Politics

It was suggested, by Guy Cramer, president of the aerospace science company United Dynamics Corp, that the full Moon might have influenced voter behavior in the US 2000 Presidential Election.[34]

Stock market

Several studies have argued that the stock market’s average returns are much higher during the half of the month closest to the new moon than the half closest to the full moon. The reasons for this have not been studied, but the authors suggest this may be due to lunar influences on mood.[35][36][37] Another study has found contradictory results and questioned these claims.[38]

Meta-analyses

A meta-analysis of thirty-seven studies that examined relationships between the Moon's four phases and human behavior revealed no significant correlation. The authors found that, of twenty-three studies that had claimed to show correlation, nearly half contained at least one statistical error.[1][3] Similarly, in a review of twenty studies examining correlations between Moon phase and suicides, most of the twenty studies found no correlation, and the ones that did report positive results were inconsistent with each other.[3] A 1978 review of the literature also found that lunar phases and human behavior are not related.[39]

Sleep quality

A July 2013 study carried out at the University of Basel in Switzerland suggests a correlation between the full Moon and human sleep quality.[40] Professor Cajochen and colleagues presented evidence that a lunar rhythm can modulate sleep structure in humans when measured under the highly controlled conditions of a circadian laboratory study protocol without time cues. Studying 33 volunteer subjects, the researchers found that subjective and objective measures of sleep varied according to lunar phase and thus may reflect human circalunar rhythmicity. Stringently controlled laboratory conditions, in a cross-sectional setting, were employed to exclude confounding effects such as increased light at night or the potential bias in perception. Measures of lunar influence on sleep structure, electroencephalographic activity during non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM), and secretion of the hormones melatonin and cortisol, were retrospectively analyzed. At no point, during and after the study, were volunteers or investigators aware of the posteriori analysis relative to lunar phase. Around full Moon it was found that electroencephalogram (EEG) delta activity during NREM sleep, an indicator of deep sleep, decreased by 30%, time to fall asleep increased by five minutes, and EEG-assessed total sleep duration was reduced by 20 minutes. These changes were associated with a decrease in subjective sleep quality and diminished endogenous melatonin levels.[40] Cajochen said: "The lunar cycle seems to influence human sleep, even when one does not 'see' the Moon and is not aware of the actual Moon phase."[41]

There are suggestions that the 2013 Cajochen study is faulty because of a relatively small (n=33) sample size and inappropriate controls for age and sex.[42] A 2014 study with larger sample sizes (n1=366, n2=29, n3=870) and better experimental controls found no effect of the lunar phase on sleep quality metrics.[42] A 2015 study of 795 children found a three-minute increase in sleep duration near the full moon,[43] but a 2016 study of 5,812 children found a five-minute decrease in sleep duration near the full moon.[44] No other modification in activity behaviors were reported,[44] and the lead scientist concluded: "Our study provides compelling evidence that the moon does not seem to influence people's behavior."[45]

In animals

California grunion fish have an unusual mating and spawning ritual during the spring and summer months. The egg laying takes place on four consecutive nights, beginning on the nights of the full and new Moons, when tides are highest.[46] However, this is a well understood reproductive strategy that is more related to tides than it is to lunar phase. It happens to correlate with the lunar phase because tides are highest when the Sun, Earth, and Moon are aligned, i.e., at new Moon or full Moon.

Correlation between hormonal changes in the testis and lunar periodicity was found in streamlined spinefoot (a type of fish), which spawns synchronously around the last Moon quarter.[47] In orange-spotted spinefoot, lunar phases affect the levels of melatonin in the blood.[47]

Evidence for lunar effect in reptiles, birds and mammals is scant,[47] but among reptiles marine iguanas (which live in the Galápagos Islands) time their trips to the sea in order to arrive at low tide.[48]

In insects, the lunar cycle may affect hormonal changes early in phylogenesis.[47] The body weight of honeybees peaks during new Moon.[47] The midge Clunio marinus has a biological clock synchronized with the moon.[40][49]

Spawning of coral Platygyra lamellina occurs at night during the summer on a date determined by the phase of the Moon; in the Red Sea, this is the three- to five-day period around the new Moon in July and the similar period in August.[50] Acropora coral time their simultaneous release of sperm and eggs to just one or two days a year, after sundown with a full moon.[51]

The Palolo worm that lives in the seas of Indonesia and Polynesia lose the terminal part of their bodies during a waning moon at a certain time of year. These parts float to the surface and release sperm and eggs. The terminal parts are gathered by people as a special food. The event would be predicted by the local priests, and the lunar calendar was set by the event.[52] Because the Palolo adjust their spawning time between October and November, and because of inter-island differences in spawning times, there are factors other than the Moon that control the timing. Such factor may include seawater temperatures, tides, weather, or other biological signals.[53]

A relationship between the moon and the birth rate of cows was reported in a 2016 study.[54]

In 2000, a retrospective study in the United Kingdom reported an association between the full moon and significant increases in animal bites to humans. The study reported that patients presenting to the A&E with injuries stemming from bites from an animal rose significantly at the time of a full moon in the period 1997-1999. The study concluded that animals have an increased inclination to bite a human during a full moon period.[55]

In plants

The popular belief that the moon has an effect on plants is unsubstantiated. When witnessed, the effects have been indirectly attributed to "moon gardeners"' attentive care.[56]

Serious doubts have been raised[57] about the claim that a species of Ephedra synchronizes its pollination peak to the full moon in July.[58]

As reported on the Australian Broadcasting Corporation's Gardening Australia, it is said that ripe passionfruit can drop more around the full moon: "The full moon effect ... at many full moons we get a really heavy drop of passionfruit, it's an industry wide effect and nobody knows why it happens."[59]

Proposed explanations

Tidal

Believers in the lunar theory suggest several different mechanisms by which the behaviour of the Moon could influence the behaviour of human beings. A common suggestion is that, since the Moon affects large bodies of water such as the ocean (a phenomenon known as "tidal force"), the Moon should be expected to have an analogous effect on human beings, whose bodies contain a great deal of water.[5][60] However, this is a misconception that fails to take into account differences in scale. The tidal force is in fact very weak and should be expected to exercise no more gravitational pull on the human body than a mosquito.[5] Besides this, the "suggestion" failed to account for the dependence of tides from both the phase of the Moon and the time of day.

Believers (David Tredinnick being a prominent example) often support their claims by noting that many police officers, teachers, and nurses have observed a lunar effect in the course of their work. To the extent that nurses and police officers do indeed claim to observe patterns, this is most likely to be explained in terms of confirmation bias: People notice if something dramatic happens during a full Moon, but do not notice when nothing dramatic happens;[60][61] furthermore, dramatic occurrences that do not occur during full Moons are typically not counted as evidence against the belief.[5] Believers are further bolstered in their belief through communal reinforcement: The more people talk about the effect, the more people notice spurious relationships.[5]

Evolutionary

Nocturnal carnivores are widely believed to have played an important role in human evolution, driving the need for nighttime shelter, the control of fire and our innate fear of darkness. We performed an extensive analysis of predatory behavior across the lunar cycle on the largest dataset of lion attacks ever assembled and found that African lions are as sensitive to moonlight when hunting humans as when hunting herbivores and that lions are most dangerous to humans when the Moon is faint or below the horizon.

C. Packer; A. Swanson; D. Ikanda & H. Kushnir (2011), "Fear of Darkness, the Full Moon and the Nocturnal Ecology of African Lions", PLoS ONE, 6 (7): e22285, Bibcode:2011PLoSO...622285P, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022285, PMC 3140494, PMID 21799812

For some 3–4 million years, bipedal hominins in the East African Rift valley were evolving in potential conflict and competition with fearsome carnivores including sabre-toothed cats equipped with excellent night-vision. Using the largest data set ever recorded – 1,000 lion attacks on humans across Tanzania between 1988 and 2009 – Craig Packer and his colleagues showed that there is a peak of attacks by lions upon humans during the evening dark hours following full Moon. According to Packer, this may help explain why so many myths and superstitions attribute fearsome dangers and nightmarish potencies to the Moon.[62] While not all archaeologists accept that lunar periodicity was ever relevant to human evolution, those favouring the idea include Curtis Marean, who heads excavations at the important Middle Stone Age site of Pinnacle Point, South Africa. Marean argues that anatomically modern humans around 165,000 years ago – when inland regions of the continent were dry, arid and uninhabitable – became restricted to small populations clustered around coastal refugia, reliant on marine resources including shellfish whose safe harvesting at spring low tides presupposed careful tracking of lunar phase. Against this background, if Marean is right, humans who ignored or misread the Moon might frequently have been drowned.[63]

With gradual offshore platforms during spring low tides, substantial areas of the intertidal zone are revealed, and these are the most productive and safest shellfish collecting times... Foragers should schedule visits to coastal residential sites at times during the lunar month when spring tides are present and then move slightly inland during neaps to broaden the size of the exploitable terrestrial area.

Marean, C. 2010. "Pinnacle Point Cave 13B (Western Cape Province, South Africa) in context: The Cape Floral kingdom, shellfish, and modern human origins". Journal of Human Evolution 59: 425e–443.

Positive ions

A further suggestion is that positive ions increase in abundance during a full Moon and that this should be expected to influence human behavior. However, this is a pseudo-scientific claim. Not only is the increase in frequency extremely slight (much smaller than that caused by air conditioning and air pollution),[60] but ionic charge – positive or negative – has no effect on human behavior, and no physiological effect other than static electric shock.[64]

Studies have shown that with regard to certain types of depression such as seasonal affective disorder (SAD), negative air ionization therapy has no statistically significant benefits beyond placebo. Moreover, such treatments require ionization many orders of magnitude greater than any differences produced by the lunar cycle, and there are superior effects found by using similar treatments with bright light instead of ion generators.[65]

War/Raids

Another theory is that in the history in human conflict, the thought is that if a group were to launch a night time attack, it would be during a full moon when one could see best.[66] During World War II, the Allies needed the light of the full moon to conduct night time operations [67] and Native American music exists specifically about full moon wars.[68]

See also

References

  1. Rotton, James; Kelly, I. W. (1985). "Much ado about the full moon: A meta-analysis of lunar-lunacy research". Psychological Bulletin. 97 (2): 286–306. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.97.2.286. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 3885282.
  2. Martens, R.; Kelly, I. W.; Saklofske, D. H. (1988). "Lunar Phase and Birthrate: A 50-year Critical Review". Psychological Reports. 63 (3): 923–934. doi:10.2466/pr0.1988.63.3.923. ISSN 0033-2941. PMID 3070616.
  3. Kelly, Ivan; Rotton, James; Culver, Roger (1986), "The Moon Was Full and Nothing Happened: A Review of Studies on the Moon and Human Behavior", Skeptical Inquirer, 10 (2): 129–43. Reprinted in The Hundredth Monkey – and other paradigms of the paranormal, edited by Kendrick Frazier, Prometheus Books. Revised and updated in The Outer Edge: Classic Investigations of the Paranormal, edited by Joe Nickell, Barry Karr, and Tom Genoni, 1996, CSICOP.
  4. Foster, Russell G.; Roenneberg, Till (2008). "Human Responses to the Geophysical Daily, Annual and Lunar Cycles". Current Biology. 18 (17): R784–R794. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.003. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 18786384.
  5. Carroll, Robert Todd (12 August 2011). "Full Moon and Lunar Effects". The Skeptic's Dictionary. Retrieved 22 October 2011.
  6. Chiazze, Leonard (1968). "The Length and Variability of the Human Menstrual Cycle". JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 203 (6): 377. doi:10.1001/jama.1968.03140060001001. ISSN 0098-7484. PMID 5694118.
  7. Adams, Cecil (24 September 1999). "What's the link between the moon and menstruation?". The Straight Dope. Retrieved 14 December 2011.
  8. William A. Gutsch (1997). 1001 things everyone should know about the universe (1st ed.). New York: Doubleday. p. 57. ISBN 9780385482233.
  9. Barash, David P.; Lipton, Judith Eve (2009). "Synchrony and Its Discontents". How women got their curves and other just-so stories evolutionary enigmas ([Online-Ausg.]. ed.). New York: Columbia University Press. ISBN 9780231518390.
  10. Abell, George; Greenspan, Bennett (1979), "The Moon and the Maternity Ward", Skeptical Inquirer, 3 (4): 17–25 Reprinted in Paranormal Borderlands of Science, edited by Kendrick Frazier, Prometheus Books, ISBN 0-87975-148-7.
  11. Abell G.O. & Greenspan B. (1979). "Human Births and the Phase of the Moon". New England Journal of Medicine. 300 (2): 96. doi:10.1056/NEJM197901113000223. ISSN 0028-4793. PMID 758594.
  12. Joshi, Raksha; Bharadwaj, Anoopendra; Gallousis, Spiro; Matthews, Ronald (1998). "Labor ward workload waxes and wanes with the lunar cycle, myth or reality?". Primary Care Update for OB/GYNS. 5 (4): 184. doi:10.1016/S1068-607X(98)00100-0. ISSN 1068-607X. PMID 10838345.
  13. Morton-Pradhan, Susan; Bay, R. Curtis; Coonrod, Dean V. (2005). "Birth rate and its correlation with the lunar cycle and specific atmospheric conditions". American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 192 (6): 1970–1973. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2005.02.066. ISSN 0002-9378. PMID 15970864.
  14. Arliss, Jill M.; Kaplan, Erin N.; Galvin, Shelley L. (2005). "The effect of the lunar cycle on frequency of births and birth complications". American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 192 (5): 1462–1464. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2004.12.034. ISSN 0002-9378. PMID 15902138.
  15. Staboulidou, Ismini; Soergel, Philipp; Vaske, Bernhard; Hillemanns, Peter (2008). "The influence of lunar cycle on frequency of birth, birth complications, neonatal outcome and the gender: A retrospective analysis". Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica. 87 (8): 875–879. doi:10.1080/00016340802233090. ISSN 0001-6349. PMID 18607814.
  16. Caton, Dan (2001). "Natality and the Moon Revisited: Do Birth Rates Depend on the Phase of the Moon?" (PDF). Bulletin of the American Astronomical Society. 33 (4): 1371.
  17. Kelly, I. W.; Martens, R. (1994). "Geophysical Variables and Behavior: LXXVIII. Lunar Phase and Birthrate: An Update". Psychological Reports. 75 (1): 507–511. doi:10.2466/pr0.1994.75.1.507. ISSN 0033-2941. PMID 7809325.
  18. Roman, Eva Maria; Soriano, German; Fuentes, Mercedes; Galvez, Maria Luz; Fernandez, Clotilde (2004). "The influence of the full moon on the number of admissions related to gastrointestinal bleeding". International Journal of Nursing Practice. 10 (6): 292–296. doi:10.1111/j.1440-172x.2004.00492.x. ISSN 1322-7114. PMID 15544585.
  19. Margot, JL (2015). "No evidence of purported lunar effect on hospital admission rates or birth rates". Nursing Research. 64 (3): 168–73. doi:10.1097/nnr.0000000000000086. PMC 4418782. PMID 25756232.
  20. Hansard, 14 Oct 2009 : Column 414
  21. Ian Douglas (11 October 2010). "MPs believe the funniest things". Daily Telegraph. Retrieved 18 October 2010.
  22. Drum, M.; Terry, C.; Hammonds, C. (1986). "Lunar phase and acting-out behaviour". Psychological Reports. 59 (2 Pt 2): 987–990. doi:10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.987. PMID 3809355.
  23. Lieber, A. (1978). "Human aggression and the lunar synodic cycle". Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 39 (5): 385–392. PMID 641019.
  24. Owen, C.; Tarantello, C.; Jones, M.; Tennant, C. (1998). "Lunar cycles and violent behaviour". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 32 (4): 496–499. doi:10.3109/00048679809068322. PMID 9711362.
  25. Barr, W. (2000). "Lunacy revisited: The influence of the moon on mental health and quality of life". Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Service. 38 (5): 28–36. PMID 10820695.
  26. Baxendale, Sallie; Fisher, Jennifer (2008). "Moonstruck? The effect of the lunar cycle on seizures". Epilepsy & Behavior. 13 (3): 549–550. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.06.009. ISSN 1525-5050. PMID 18602495.
  27. Attewill, Fred (5 June 2007). "Police link full moon to aggression". The Guardian. London. Retrieved 11 May 2010.
  28. "Crackdown on lunar-fuelled crime". BBC News. 5 June 2007. Retrieved 26 July 2013.
  29. toledoblade.com – Analysis shines light on full Moon, crime
  30. Skeptic's Dictionary and Refuge: Mass Media Bunk
  31. "Police busy for full moon". The Kentucky Post. E. W. Scripps Company. 29 January 2002. Archived from the original on 6 July 2007.
  32. "Link between moon and crime supported – national". Stuff.co.nz. 7 February 2008. Retrieved 11 July 2011.
  33. Redelmeier, D; Shafir, E (11 December 2017). "The full moon and motorcycle related mortality: population based double control study". BMJ. 359.
  34. #Y127; 24% of the U.S. Presidential Vote swayed by the Full Moon effect
  35. Yuan, Kathy Zhichao; Zheng, Lu; Zhu, Qiaoqiao (2001). "Are Investors Moonstruck? - Lunar Phases and Stock Returns". Journal of Empirical Finance. 13 (1): 1–23. doi:10.1016/j.jempfin.2005.06.001. SSRN 283156.
  36. Yuan, Kathy; Zheng, Lu; Zhu, Qiaoqiao (2006). "Are investors moonstruck? Lunar phases and stock returns". Journal of Empirical Finance. 13 (1): 1–23. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.736.9528. doi:10.1016/j.jempfin.2005.06.001. ISSN 0927-5398.
  37. Dichev, Ilia D.; Janes, Troy D. (2001). "Lunar Cycle Effects in Stock Returns". SSRN 281665.
  38. Herbst, Anthony F. (2007). "Lunacy in the Stock Market—What is the Evidence?". Journal of Bioeconomics. 9 (1): 1–18. doi:10.1007/s10818-007-9016-3. ISSN 1387-6996.
  39. Campbell, D.E.; Beets, J.L. (1978). "Lunacy and the Moon". Psychological Bulletin. 85 (5): 1123–1129. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.85.5.1123. PMID 704720.
  40. Cajochen, Christian; Altanay-Ekici, Songül; Münch, Mirjam; Frey, Sylvia; Knoblauch, Vera; Wirz-Justice, Anna (2013). "Evidence that the Lunar Cycle Influences Human Sleep". Current Biology. 23 (15): 1485–1488. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.029. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 23891110.
  41. Roberts, Michelle (25 July 2013). "BBC News – Full Moon 'disturbs a good night's sleep'". BBC News. Bbc.co.uk. Retrieved 26 July 2013.
  42. Cordi, Maren; Ackermann, Sandra; Bes, Frederik W.; Hartmann, Francina; Konrad, Boris N.; Genzel, Lisa; Pawlowski, Marcel; Steiger, Axel; Schulz, Hartmut; Rasch, Björn; Dresler, Martin (2014). "Lunar cycle effects on sleep and the file drawer problem". Current Biology. 24 (12): R549–R550. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.017. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 24937275.
  43. Sjödin, A.; Hjorth, M. F.; Damsgaard, C. T.; Ritz, C.; Astrup, A.; Michaelsen, K. F. (April 2015). "Physical activity, sleep duration and metabolic health in children fluctuate with the lunar cycle: science behind the myth". Clinical Obesity. 5 (2): 60–66. doi:10.1111/cob.12092. PMC 4672692. PMID 25808903.
  44. Chaput, Jean-Philippe; Weippert, Madyson; LeBlanc, Allana G.; Hjorth, Mads F.; Michaelsen, Kim F.; Katzmarzyk, Peter T.; Tremblay, Mark S.; Barreira, Tiago V.; Broyles, Stephanie T.; Fogelholm, Mikael; Hu, Gang; Kuriyan, Rebecca; Kurpad, Anura; Lambert, Estelle V.; Maher, Carol; Maia, Jose; Matsudo, Victor; Olds, Timothy; Onywera, Vincent; Sarmiento, Olga L.; Standage, Martyn; Tudor-Locke, Catrine; Zhao, Pei; Sjödin, Anders M. (24 March 2016). "Are Children Like Werewolves? Full Moon and Its Association with Sleep and Activity Behaviors in an International Sample of Children". Frontiers in Pediatrics. 4: 24. doi:10.3389/fped.2016.00024. PMC 4805596. PMID 27047907.
  45. "Does the moon affect our mood or actions?". www.sciencedaily.com. Retrieved 11 May 2016.
  46. "What is a grunion?". Pepperdine University. Retrieved 17 September 2013.
  47. Zimecki, M (2006). "The lunar cycle: effects on human and animal behavior and physiology". Postepy Hig Med Dosw. 60: 1–7. PMID 16407788. Retrieved 24 July 2015.
  48. Martin Wikelski; Michaela Hau (December 1995). "Is There an Endogenous Tidal Foraging Rhythm in Marine Iguanas?". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 10 (4): 335–350. doi:10.1177/074873049501000407. PMID 8639942.
  49. Tobias Kaiser; Dietrich Neumann; David Heckel (May 2011). "Timing the tides: genetic control of diurnal and lunar emergence times is correlated in the marine midge Clunio marinus". BMC Genetics. 12: 49. doi:10.1186/1471-2156-12-49. PMC 3124415. PMID 21599938.
  50. Schlesinger, Y.; Loya, Y. (1991). Larval development and survivorship in the corals Favia favus and Platygyra lamellina. Developments in Hydrobiology. 66. pp. 101–108. doi:10.1007/978-94-011-3240-4_14. ISBN 978-94-010-5428-7.
  51. Alex Riley (20 February 2016). "Playing Cupid to get reluctant corals in the mood for love". New Scientist.
  52. Stephen Oppenheimer (1998). Eden in the East. pp. 345, 346. ISBN 978-0753806791.
  53. Craig, P. "Natural History Guide to American Samoa" (PDF). National Park of American Samoa, Department Marine and Wildlife Resources, American Samoa Community College. Retrieved 11 May 2016.
  54. Yonezawa, Tomohiro; Uchida, Mona; Tomioka, Michiko; Matsuki, Naoaki (31 August 2016). "Lunar Cycle Influences Spontaneous Delivery in Cows". PLOS ONE. 11 (8): e0161735. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1161735Y. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161735. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 5006988. PMID 27580019.
  55. Yonezawa, Bhattacharjee; Bradle, Mona; Smith, Scally; Wilson (23 December 2000). "Do animals bite more during a full moon? Retrospective observational analysis". BMJ. 321 (7276): 1559–1561. doi:10.1136/bmj.321.7276.1559. PMC 27561. PMID 11124173.
  56. Roach, John. "Age-Old Moon Gardening Growing in Popularity". National Geographic. Retrieved 15 August 2016.
  57. Margot, JL (October 2015). "Insufficient Evidence of Purported Lunar Effect on Pollination in Ephedra". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 30 (5): 454–6. doi:10.1177/0748730415591662. PMID 26316347.
  58. Rydin, C; Bolinder, K (April 2015). "Moonlight pollination in the gymnosperm Ephedra (Gnetales)". Biology Letters. 11 (4): 20140993. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.0993. PMC 4424609. PMID 25832814.
  59. Presenter: Jerry Coleby-Williams (28 September 2018). [www.abc.net.au/gardening/factsheets/passionfruit-professional/10312208 "Passionfruit Professional"] Check |url= value (help). Gardening Australia. Series 29. Episode 36. 6:22 minutes in. Australian Broadcasting Corporation.
  60. Adams, Cecil (13 March 1987). "Do things get crazy when the moon is full?". The Straight Dope. Retrieved 14 December 2011.
  61. Gilovich, Thomas (1993). How we know what isn't so : the fallibility of human reason in everyday life. New York: Free Press. ISBN 9780029117064.
  62. Packer, C.; Swanson, A.; Ikanda, D.; Kushnir, H. (2011). "Fear of Darkness, the Full Moon and the Nocturnal Ecology of African Lions". PLoS ONE. 6 (7): e22285. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...622285P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022285. PMC 3140494. PMID 21799812.
  63. Curtis Marean (2010). Pinnacle Point Cave 13B (Western Cape Province, South Africa) in context: The Cape Floral kingdom, shellfish, and modern human origins. Journal of Human Evolution 59: 425e443
  64. Novella, Steven (14 September 2011). "Pseudoscience Sells". Retrieved 20 March 2012.
  65. Flory R, Ametepe J, Bowers B (May 2010). "A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of bright light and high-density negative air ions for treatment of Seasonal Affective Disorder". Psychiatry Res. 177 (1–2): 101–8. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.08.011. PMID 20381162.
  66. http://www.illinoislighting.org/resources/ICROL-Moonlight.pdf
  67. "World War II History: D-Day the Invasion of Normandy for Kids".
  68. "Native American War Music - Full Moon War Preparation Hypnotic Shaman Music".

Bibliography

  • Abell, George (1979). Review of the book The Alleged Lunar Effect by Arnold Lieber, Skeptical Inquirer, Spring 1979, 68-73. Reprinted in Science Confronts the Paranormal, edited by Kendrick Frazier, Prometheus Books, ISBN 0-87975-314-5.
  • Abell, George and Barry Singer (1981). Science and the Paranormal - probing the existence of the supernatural, Charles Scribner's Sons, chapter 5, ISBN 0-684-17820-6.
  • Berman, Bob (2003). Fooled by the Full Moon - Scientists search for the sober truth behind some loony ideas, Discover, September 2003, page 30.
  • Caton, Dan (2001). Natality and the Moon Revisited: Do Birth Rates Depend on the Phase of the Moon?, Bulletin of the American Astronomical Society, Vol 33, No. 4, 2001, p. 1371. A summary of the results of the paper.
  • Diefendorf, David (2007), Amazing... But false! Hundreds of "facts" you thought were true, but aren't, Sterling Publishing, ISBN 978-1-4027-3791-6
  • Foster, Russell G.; Roenneberg, Till (2008). "Human Responses to the Geophysical Daily, Annual and Lunar Cycles". Current Biology. 18 (17): R784–R794. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.003. PMID 18786384. Lunar cycles had, and continue to have, an influence upon human culture, though despite a persistent belief that our mental health and other behaviours are modulated by the phase of the moon, there is no solid evidence that human biology is in any way regulated by the lunar cycle
  • Packer, C.; Swanson, A.; Ikanda, D.; Kushnir, H. (2011). "Fear of Darkness, the Full Moon and the Nocturnal Ecology of African Lions". PLoS ONE. 6 (7): 22285. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...622285P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022285. PMC 3140494. PMID 21799812.
  • Palmer, JD; Udry, JR; Morris, NM (1982). "Diurnal and weekly, but no lunar rhythms in humans copulation". Human Biology; an International Record of Research. 54 (1): 111–21. PMID 7200945.
  • Sanduleak, Nicholas (1985). The Moon is Acquitted of Murder in Cleveland, Skeptical Inquirer, Spring 1985, 236-42. Reprinted in Science Confronts the Paranormal, edited by Kendrick Frazier, Prometheus Books, ISBN 0-87975-314-5.
  • Zimecki, Michał (2006). "The lunar cycle: effects on human and animal behavior and physiology". Postepy Higieny I Medycyny Doswiadczalnej. 60: 1–7. PMID 16407788. In fish the lunar clock influences reproduction and involves the hypothalamus-pituitary-gonadal axis. In birds, the daily variations in melatonin and corticosterone disappear during full-moon days. The lunar cycle also exerts effects on laboratory rats with regard to taste sensitivity and the ultrastructure of pineal gland cells. Cyclic variations related to the moon's phases in the magnitude of the humoral immune response of mice to polivinylpyrrolidone and sheep erythrocytes were also described. It is suggested that melatonin and endogenous steroids may mediate the described cyclic alterations of physiological processes. The release of neurohormones may be triggered by the electromagnetic radiation and/or the gravitational pull of the moon

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.