Frown

A frown (also known as a scowl) is a facial expression in which the eyebrows are brought together, and the forehead is wrinkled, usually indicating displeasure, sadness or worry, or less often confusion or concentration. The appearance of a frown varies by culture. Although most technical definitions define it as a wrinkling of the brow, in North America it is primarily thought of as an expression of the mouth. In those cases when used iconically, as with an emoticon, it is entirely presented by the curve of the lips forming a down-open curve. The mouth expression is also commonly referred to in the colloquial English phrase "turn that frown upside down" which indicates changing from sad to happy.

In his 16th-century portrait of an unidentified man, Albrecht Dürer powerfully captures personality through facial expression, body language, and clothing. Work: Bildnis eines unbekannten Mannes (1521)

Description

Charles Darwin described the primary act of frowning as the furrowing of the brow which leads to a rise in the upper lip and a down-turning of the corners of the mouth.[1] While the appearance of a frown varies from culture to culture, there appears to be some degree of universality to the recognition of the frown as a negative facial expression.[2] In fact, frowning as a component of anger or disgust is widely recognized as a universal expression easily recognized across cultures.[3] This universality suggests a shared adaptive quality to frowning allowing for social communication of negative emotional states.[4]

Photographs of frowning and crying children from The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (1872) by Charles Darwin

Scott Fahlman first suggested the use of the colon with the left parenthesis to iconically represent a frowning face on the Internet in what has become a well-known emoticon.[5] In this form the frown is entirely presented as a curve of the lips facing away from the eyes. Specifically, frowns that incorporate the furrowing of the brow are a response to perceived obstacles to the achievement of goals, while frowns that involve movement of the cheeks reflect an unpleasant reaction.[6] Just as smiling alone can make one feel better,[7] frowning can make one feel worse. In a scientific study participants that held their face in a frown ranked images as more unpleasant than participants who viewed the images with a neutral facial expression.[8] In a similar test, participants reported increased anger with the manipulated expression of a frown and they also ranked cartoons they saw as less funny than participants with the manipulated expression of a smile.[9]

Social behavior

In social settings frowns are most frequently used to express a range of negative emotions including anger and displeasure. However there are social differences that dictate who is allowed to frown in any given social setting. Socially dominant individuals are more likely to frown than those of a lower social status. Individuals with a high social status are permitted to display their emotions more freely, while low dominance individuals are expected to show signs of affiliation. [10] It is considered more socially acceptable for men to display negative emotions than women; women also tend to be less likely to frown than men.[11] Unlike smiling, frowns do not appear to be directly socially contagious; seeing someone frown does not necessarily make you frown.[12] However, in a scientific study participants that were subliminally exposed to frowning faces, were more likely to react with a frown when later shown a neutral face.[13] Frowns can also increase counter-empathy reactions. When the loser of a competition frowns it increases brain responses associated with pleasure in the winner, which is possibly related to the feeling of schadenfreude experienced when witnessing the misfortune of others.[14] Frowns also contribute to impressions formed even during brief encounters with other individuals. When shown a 30-second video clip (without audio) of a frowning college professor, students tend to rate the professor poorly on evaluations.[15]

Muscles used

Illustration of facial muscles and other tissue of the lateral human head and neck

It is a long-held belief that it takes more muscles to frown than it does to smile.[16] It is difficult to determine exactly how many muscles are involved in smiling or frowning as there is a wide range of facial expressions that might be considered a frown or a smile. At minimum ten muscles are required to smile in which only the upper lip and corners of the mouth are lifted.[17] A similarly minimal frown requires only six muscles to lower the corners of the mouth.[17] According to plastic surgeon Dr. David H. Song of the University of Chicago Medical Center, however, frowning requires 11 muscles while smiling requires 12.[18] This method of counting the number of muscles used in generating a facial expression does not take into account the energy consumed by each muscle or the individual variability in facial muscles. While humans share facial musculature to express the universal emotions, some humans have more muscles in their face and may use more of them when smiling or frowning.[19]

Muscles of facial expression[18]
FrowningSmiling
Platysma (2 muscles)Zygomaticus major (2 muscles)
Orbicularis oculi (2 muscles)Orbicularis oculi (2 muscles)
Corrugator supercilii (2 muscles)Levator labii superioris (2 muscles)
Procerus (1 muscle)Levator anguli oris (2 muscles)
Orbicularis oris (1 muscle)Risorius (2 muscles)
Mentalis (1 muscle)Zygomaticus minor (2 muscles)
Depressor anguli oris (2 muscles)
11 muscles total12 muscles total

See also

References

  1. Darwin, Charles R. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray. pp. 148–152.
  2. Russell, J.A. (1994). "Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expressions? A review of the cross-cultural studies". Psychological Bulletin. 115 (1): 102–141. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.102. PMID 8202574.
  3. Paul Ekman, Emotions Revealed: Recognizing Faces and Feelings to Improve Communication, Holt Paperbacks, ISBN 0-8050-7516-X, 2004, p.63
  4. Izard, Carroll E. (1994). "Innate and Universal Facial Expressions: Evidence From Developmental and Cross-Cultural Research". Psychological Bulletin. 115 (2): 288–299. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.288. PMID 8165273.
  5. -) turns 25, Associated Press, 2007-09-20, archived from the original on 2007-10-12, retrieved 2007-09-20
  6. Pope, L.; Smith, C. (1994). "On the distinct meanings of smiles and frowns". Cognition & Emotion. 8: 65–72. doi:10.1080/02699939408408929.
  7. Strack, Fritz; Martin, L.; Stepper, S. (1988). "Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A non-obtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 54 (5): 768–777. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768. PMID 3379579.
  8. Larsen, R.; Kasimatis, M.; Frey, K. (1992). "Facilitating the Furrowed Brow: An Unobtrusive Test of the Facial Feedback Hypothesis Applied to Unpleasant Affect". Cognition & Emotion. 6 (5): 321–338. doi:10.1080/02699939208409689. PMID 29022461.
  9. Smith, Craig A. (Mar 1989). "Dimensions of appraisal and physiological response in emotion". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56 (3): 339–353. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.3.339.
  10. LaFrance, M.; Hect, M.A. (1999). "Option or obligation to smile: The effects of power and gender on facial expression.". In P.Phillipot; R.S. Feldman; E.J. Coars (eds.). The social context of nonverbal behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 45–70. ISBN 978-0-521-58666-5.
  11. Hess, Ursula; Adams, R.B.; Kleck, R.E. (2005). "Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance, affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger" (PDF). Cognition and Emotion. 19 (4): 515–536. doi:10.1080/02699930441000364.
  12. Hinsz, V.B.; Tomhave, J.A. (1991). "Smile and (Half) the World Smiles with You, Frown and You Frown Alone". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 17 (5): 586–592. doi:10.1177/0146167291175014. S2CID 33343894.
  13. Ulf, Dimberg; Thunberg, M.; Elmehed, K. (January 2000). "Unconscious Facial Reactions to Emotional Facial Expressions". Psychological Science. 11 (1): 86–89. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00221. PMID 11228851.
  14. Yamada, M.; Lamm, C.; Decety, J. (2011). "Pleasing frowns, disappointing smiles: An ERP investigation of counterempathy". Emotion. 11 (6): 1336–1345. doi:10.1037/a0023854. PMID 21728414.
  15. Ambady, Nalini; Rosenthal, R. (1993). "Half a Minute: Predicting Teacher Evaluations From Thin Slices of Nonverbal Behavior and Physical Attractiveness". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 64 (3): 431–441. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.431.
  16. Hix, John (1931). Strange As It Seems. New York: Sears Publishing Company. p. 224.
  17. Scheve, Tom. "How many muscles does it take to smile?". Discovery Fit & Health. Retrieved 29 August 2011.
  18. Adams, Cecil. "Does it take fewer muscles to smile than it does to frown?". The Straight Dope. Retrieved 26 August 2011.
  19. Waller, Bridget M.; Cray, James J.; Burrows, Anne M. (2008). "Selection for Universal Facial Emotion". Emotion. 8 (3): 435–439. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.612.9868. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.435. PMID 18540761.
  • Media related to frowning at Wikimedia Commons
  • The dictionary definition of scowl at Wiktionary
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.