Social experiment

A social experiment is a kind of psychological or sociological research for testing people’s reaction to certain situations or events. The experiment relies on a particular social approach, when a main source of information is people with their own knowledge and point of view. To carry out a social experiment, specialists usually divide participants into two groups — active participants (people who take action in some event) and respondents (people who react to the action). Throughout the period of experiment, participants are monitored by specialists to identify the effects and differences as a result of the experiment.

Social experiments have also been used by companies to collect data about consumers and their opinion about a product or some particular topic.[1]

History

In 1895, American psychologist Norman Triplett constructed one of the earliest known social experiments, in which he found out that cyclists managed to ride a bike faster when racing against another person rather than racing against the clock. He duplicated the experiment in a laboratory using children and fishing reels and received similar results. [2] Field social experiments had proved to be efficient as it reflects real life due to its natural setting.[3]

The social experiments that are commonly referred to today were conducted decades later, in which an experiment is done in a controlled environment such as a Laboratory. A famous example of which is Stanley Milgram's obedience experiment in 1963. Social experiments began in the United States as a test of the negative income tax concept in the late 1960s and since then have been conducted on all the populated continents.

During the 1970s, criticism of the ethics and accusations of gender and racial bias led to a reassessment of both the field of social psychology and the conducted experiments. While experimental methods were still employed, other methods gained popularity.[4]

Ethics

Social experimentation has raised many ethical concerns, due to its manipulation of large groups of the population, often without the consent or knowledge of the subjects.[5] In certain cases, Social experimentation has been staged unknown to the viewer in order to promote the image of the individual or for the pure purpose of generating controversy.[6]

Researchers also believed that the impact of Informal Social Experiments via social media videos may have negative consequences on formal social marketing research as well as the society in general,[7] detailing that while Informal Social Experiments addresses moral and social issues such as child safety, self-confidence etc, producers of these social experiments do it for their own gain and benefits.[7]

Well-known social experiments

Bystander Apathy (Effect)

Based around the murder of Kitty Genovese right outside her home, The New York Times stated that there were 38 witnesses that either saw or heard the fatal stabbing take place, and not a single person came to her aid. Although this number was proven to be exaggerated, this murder was known as what was coined "bystander apathy" by social psychologists Bibb Latané and John Darley in 1968.[8]

For their experiment, Latané and Darley [8] tried to replicate the Genovese slaying by having participants aware of each other but unable to communicate directly. Each participant was in a cubical in contact with each other via a microphone, however only one voice was allowed to speak at a time. A taped recording played of a participant having an epileptic seizure. When the participant believed themselves to be alone they invariably attempted to find help. When the participant believed others were around the speed and frequency of response declined significantly. The authors concluded that situational factors play an influential role in bystander apathy. People are less likely to help in an emergency if other people are present. Two reasons were offered by Latané and Darley: first is diffusion of responsibility. The second is pluralistic ignorance or the mentality that if nobody else is helping, then I am not needed as well.

Current research on bystander apathy by psychologist Kyle Thomas et al. found that people's decisions to help are influenced by their level of knowledge. While diffusion of responsibility and pluralistic ignorance are factors, the researchers found that bystanders’ decisions also consider what they know about other bystanders and the situation before getting involved.[9]

HighScope

The HighScope Perry Preschool Project was evaluated in a randomized controlled trial of 123 children (58 were randomly assigned to a treatment group that received the program and a control group of 65 children that did not). Prior to the program, the preschool and control groups were equivalent in measures of intellectual performance and demographic characteristics. After the program, the educational and life outcomes for the children receiving the program were much superior to outcomes for the children not receiving the program. Many of the program effects were significant or approaching significance.[10][11] At age 40, the participants were interviewed once more, and school, social services, and arrest records were pulled. The participants had higher earning jobs, committed fewer crimes, and were more likely to hold a job than adults who did not attend preschool.

RAND Health Insurance Experiment

The RAND Health Insurance Experiment was an experimental study of health care costs, utilization, and outcomes in the United States, which assigned people randomly to different kinds of plans and followed their behavior, from 1974 to 1982. As a result, it provided stronger evidence than studies that examine people afterwards who were not randomly assigned. It concluded that cost sharing reduced "inappropriate or unnecessary" medical care (overutilization), but also reduced "appropriate or needed" medical care. It did not have enough statistical power to tell whether people who got less appropriate or needed care were more likely to die as a result.

Oportunidades/Prospera/Progresa

Opportunidades (now rebranded as "Prospera") is a government social assistance (welfare) program in Mexico founded in 2002, based on a previous program called Progresa, created in 1997.[12] It is designed to target poverty by providing cash payments to families in exchange for regular school attendance, health clinic visits, and nutrition support.[13] Oportunidades is credited with decreasing poverty and improving health and educational attainment in regions where it has been deployed.[14]

Moving to Opportunity

Moving to Opportunity for Fair Housing was a randomized social experiment sponsored by the United States Department of Housing and Urban Development in the 1990s among 4600 low-income families with children living in high-poverty public housing projects. The program was designed based on the assumption that households benefit from living in higher-opportunity neighborhoods. Early evaluations of the MTO program, however, showed minimal gains for participant families. One explanation for these findings is the short length of time which MTO families typically spent in lower-poverty neighborhoods; the positive effects of longer-term exposure to low-poverty neighborhoods appear more promising.[15]

Stanford prison experiment

The Stanford prison experiment was a study of the psychological effects of becoming a prisoner or prison guard. The experiment was conducted at Stanford University on August 14–20, 1971, by a team of researchers led by psychology professor Philip Zimbardo using college students.[16] It was funded by the U.S. Office of Naval Research[17] and was of interest to both the U.S. Navy and Marine Corps as an investigation into the causes of conflict between military guards and prisoners. The experiment is a classic study on the psychology of imprisonment[18] and is a topic covered in most introductory psychology textbooks.[19]

Experiments by Muzafer Sherif

Sherif was a founder of modern social psychology, who developed several unique and powerful techniques for understanding social processes, particularly social norms, and social conflict. Sherif's experimental study of the autokinetic movement demonstrated how mental evaluation norms were created by human beings.[20] Sherif is equally famous for the Robbers Cave Experiments. This series of experiments, begun in Connecticut and concluded in Oklahoma, took boys from intact middle-class families, who were carefully screened to be psychologically normal, delivered them to a summer camp setting (with researchers doubling as counselors) and created social groups that came into conflict with each other.

Bobo doll experiment

The Bobo doll experiment was a study carried out by Albert Bandura who was a professor at Stanford University. It focused on the study of aggression using three groups of preschoolers as the subjects. Bandura took inflatable plastic toys called Bobo dolls and weighted them down to always stand upright. The preschoolers were divided into three groups by gender, and then into six subgroups. One of the groups would observe an adult act aggressively towards the Bobo doll, another group would observe an adult with non-aggressive behaviors, and the last group would not be exposed to any behavior models. The study found the preschoolers exposed to the aggressive behavior had imitated the aggressiveness towards the doll, regardless of gender. The other two groups showed significantly less hostility towards the doll. The study had shown aggressive and non-aggressive behaviors were learned by observing others and had a significant effect on the subjects even after the study was concluded.[21]

Stanford marshmallow experiment

The Stanford marshmallow experiment was a study done by psychologist Walter Mischel on delayed gratification in the early 1970s. During the three studies, In these studies, a child was offered a choice between one small reward provided immediately or two small rewards if they waited for a short period, approximately 15 minutes, during which the tester left the room and then returned. (The reward was sometimes a marshmallow, but often a cookie or a pretzel.) In follow-up studies, the researchers found that children who were able to wait longer for the preferred rewards tended to have better life outcomes, as measured by SAT scores, educational attainment, body mass index (BMI), and other life measures.[22]

Asch Conformity experiment

The Asch experiment took place in the Swarthmore College in 1951. Solomon Asch conducted an experiment to investigate the extent where social pressure from a majority group could affect a person to confirm.[23] Asch took 50 people from the college to participate in a vision test. They were paired up with 7 other people who they believed to be random, but instead were part of a control group who would choose the same answers. The real participant would give his or her answers last. Out of the 18 trials, the group gave the wrong answers 12 times. 75% of the participants conformed once or more, and the remaining 25% never conformed to the group's wrong answers. Participants were interviewed after the experiment, and although they knew the answers were wrong, they confirmed to not be ridiculed by the group. A few individuals said they thought the group's answers were correct. Asch concluded that people confirmed because they either wanted to fit in or thought the group was right.

Hawthorne experiment

The Hawthorne experiment took place in 1924 in the city of Chicago. Elton Mayo is widely known as the person behind the project. However, his involvement started in 1928 after he was invited by George Pennock, the Assistant Works Manager for the Hawthorne plant of Western Electric. During the experiment, workers were separated into two groups to study the effects of different incentives on their productivity. Different variations were tested, such as changing the light levels in the rooms. Other, more obvious incentives, such as monetary incentives and rest pauses, were also tested and seemed to show positive results.[24] Several conclusions were made, after the experiments finished:

  • When employees had more freedom to choose their own conditions and output standards, their productivity increased;
  • Social interaction played an important role in the creation of a high level of group cohesion;
  • People tend to put more effort when they feel their worth and cooperate with each other.[24]

Informal social experiments

References

  1. "10 marketing experiments to improve your reach on social media". Learn. 2019-02-26. Retrieved 2019-07-19.
  2. "Social Facilitation | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2019-07-19.
  3. "Experimental Methods in Psychology | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2019-07-19.
  4. T D Cook; Shadish, and W. R. (1994). "Social Experiments: Some Developments over the Past Fifteen Years". Annual Review of Psychology. 45 (1): 545–580. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.002553.
  5. Humphreys, Macartan (2015-06-01). "Reflections on the Ethics of Social Experimentation". Journal of Globalization and Development. 6 (1). doi:10.1515/jgd-2014-0016. ISSN 1948-1837.
  6. Chen, Ted; Klien, Asher. "Over Hoax Claims, Homeless Man and YouTuber Insist Their Viral Video Is Genuine". NBC (2/01/15). Retrieved 24 November 2019.
  7. Manna, Valerie A. (August 2017). The ethics of informal social experiments. Macromarketing Society Inc. and the Marketing Department, University of Otago. hdl:10182/9574. ISBN 978-0-473-39583-4.
  8. Latane, Bibb; John M. Darley (June 1, 1970). The Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn't He Help?. Prentice Hal. ISBN 0139386130.
  9. Thomas, Kyle A.; De Freitas, Julian; DeScioli, Peter; Pinker, Steven (2016). "Recursive Mentalizing and Common Knowledge in the Bystander Effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 145 (5): 621–629. doi:10.1037/xge0000153. PMID 26913616.
  10. Significant Benefits, The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27
  11. Coalition for Evidence-Based Policy: Social Programs That Work: Perry Preschool Project
  12. Paying for Better Parenting, New York Times, Accessed 12/07/06
  13. Mexico's Oportunidades Program Accessed 12/07/06
  14. Bulletin of the World Health Organization - Reaching Mexico's poorest Accessed 12/07/06
  15. Blumenberg, Evelyn; Pierce, Gregory (December 2017). "Car Access and Long-term Poverty Exposure: Evidence from the Moving to Opportunity (MTO) Experiment". Journal of Transport Geography. 65: 92–100. doi:10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2017.10.009.
  16. "8. Conclusion". Stanford Prison Experiment.
  17. FAQ on official site Archived 2012-09-09 at Archive.today
  18. "Archived copy". Archived from the original on 2001-11-05. Retrieved 2015-04-19.CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link)
  19. "Intro to psychology textbooks gloss over criticisms of Zimbardo's Stanford Prison Experiment". 7 September 2014.
  20. Jaan Valsiner (2001). Comparative study of human cultural development. Fundacion Infancia y Aprendizaje. ISBN 9788495264015.
  21. .https://www.britannica.com/event/Bobo-doll-experiment
  22. "Delaying Gratification" (PDF). Apa.org. Retrieved 17 December 2018.
  23. McLeod, Saul. "Asch Experiment". Simply Psychology. Retrieved 16 December 2018.
  24. "Elton Mayo: The Hawthorne Experiments Thinker". The British Library. Retrieved 2018-02-26.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.