Navigation research

Whereas originally the term Navigation applies to the process of directing a ship to a destination, Navigation research deals with fundamental aspects of navigation in general. It can be defined as "The process of determining and maintaining a course or trajectory to a goal location" (Franz, Mallot, 2000). It concerns basically all moving agents, biological or artificial, autonomous or remote-controlled.

Franz and Mallot proposed a navigation hierarchy in Robotics and Autonomous Systems 30 (2006):[1]

Behavioural prerequisiteNavigation competence
Local navigation
SearchGoal recognitionFinding the goal without active goal orientation
Direction-followingAlign course with local directionFinding the goal from one direction
AimingKeep goal in frontFinding a salient goal from a catchment area
GuidanceAttain spatial relation to the surrounding objectsFinding a goal defined by its relation to the surroundings
Way-finding
Recognition-triggered responseAssociation sensory pattern-actionFollowing fixed routes
Topological navigationRoute integration, route planningFlexible concatenation of route segments
Survey navigationEmbedding into a common reference frameFinding paths over novel terrain

There are two basic methods for navigation:

  • Egocentric navigation also known as Idiothetic navigation
  • Allocentric navigation also known as Allothetic navigation

Human navigation

In human navigation people visualize different routes in their minds to plan how to get from one place to another. The things which they rely on to plan these routes vary from person to person and are the basis of the differing navigational strategies.

Some people use measures of distance and absolute directional terms (north, south, east, and west) in order to visualize the best pathway from point to point. The use of these more general, external cues as directions is considered part of an allocentric navigation strategy. Allocentric navigation is typically seen in males and is beneficial primarily in large and/or unfamiliar environments.[2] This likely has some basis in evolution when males would have to navigate through large and unfamiliar environments while hunting.[3] The use of allocentric strategies when navigating primarily activates the hippocampus and parahippocampus in the brain. This navigation strategy relies more on a mental, spatial map than visible cues, giving it an advantage in unknown areas but a flexibility to be used in smaller environments as well. The fact that it is mainly males that favor this strategy is likely related to the generalization that males are better navigators than females as it is better able to be applied in a greater variety of settings.[2]

Egocentric navigation relies on more local landmarks and personal directions (left/right) to navigate and visualize a pathway. This reliance on more local and well-known stimuli for finding their way makes it difficult to apply in new locations, but is instead most effective in smaller, familiar environments.[2] Evolutionarily, egocentric navigation likely comes from our ancestors who would forage for their food and need to be able to return to the same places daily to find edible plants. This foraging usually occurred in relatively nearby areas and was most commonly done by the females in hunter-gatherer societies.[3] Females, today, are typically better at knowing where various landmarks are and often rely on them when giving directions. Egocentric navigation causes high levels of activation in the right parietal lobe and prefrontal regions of the brain that are involved in visuospatial processing.[2]

Robotic navigation

Outdoor robots can use GPS in a similar way to automotive navigation systems. Alternative systems can be used with floor plan instead of maps for indoor robots, combined with localization wireless hardware.

See also

  • Electric beacon
  • Visual navigation

Notes

  1. Franz, Matthias O.; Mallot, Hanspeter A. (2000). "Biomimetic robot navigation". Robotics and Autonomous Systems. 30 (1–2): 133–153. doi:10.1016/S0921-8890(99)00069-X.
  2. Andreano & Cahill (2009)
  3. Geary (1998)

References

  • Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning & memory, 16(4), 248–246.
  • Geary, D. C. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. (pp. 259–303). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.