Intellectual curiosity

Intellectual curiosity (also called epistemic curiosity) is curiosity that leads to an acquisition of general knowledge. It can include curiosity about such things as what objects are composed of, the underlying mechanisms of systems, mathematical relationships, languages, social norms, and history. It can be differentiated from another type of curiosity that does not lead to acquisition of general knowledge, such as curiosity about the intimate secrets of other people. It is a facet of openness to experience in the Five Factor Model used to describe human personalities.[1] It is similar to need for cognition and typical intellectual engagement.

History

In antiquity, the Roman philosopher Cicero wrote about our innate love of learning:

So great is our innate love of learning and of knowledge that no one can doubt that man’s nature is strongly attracted to these things even without the lure of any profit. For my part, I believe Homer had something of this sort in view in his imaginary account of the songs of the Sirens. Apparently it was not the sweetness of their voices or the novelty and diversity of their songs, but their professions of knowledge that used to attract the passing voyagers; it was the passion for learning that kept men rooted to the Sirens’ rocky shores.

Marcus Tullius Cicero, [2]

In 1738, the Scottish philosopher David Hume differentiated intellectual curiosity from a more primitive form of curiosity:

The same theory, that accounts for the love of truth in mathematics and algebra, may be extended to morals, politics, natural philosophy, and other studies, where we consider not the other abstract relations of ideas, but their real connexions and existence. But beside the love of knowledge, which displays itself in the sciences, there is a certain curiosity implanted in human nature, which is a passion derived from a quite different principle. Some people have an insatiable desire of knowing the actions and circumstances of their neighbours, though their interest be no way concerned in them, and they must entirely depend on others for their information; in which case there is no room for study or application. Let us search for the reason of this phenomenon.

David Hume, A treatise of human nature (p. 453), [3]

Later, in 1954, Berlyne differentiated it into perceptual curiosity and epistemic curiosity,[4] and in 2004 a psychometric scale to assess epistemic and perceptual curiosity was developed.[5]

Intellectual development in children

Humans seem to be born with intellectual curiosity, but depending on how parents react to questions from their children, intellectual curiosity might be increased or decreased.[6] Parents that always react negatively to questions asked by their children, are discouraging them from asking questions, and that is likely to make them less curious. On the other hand, parents that always react positively to questions asked by their children, are encouraging them to ask questions, and that is likely to make them more curious. There is a book about how to foster intellectual curiosity and a love for knowledge in children.[7]

Academic performance

It has been positively correlated with academic performance (0.20), together with general intelligence (0.35) and conscientiousness (0.20).[8]

Scientific progress

Toby E. Huff has argued that the European civilization had a high level of intellectual curiosity during the scientific revolution.[9] He also argues that other civilizations have had a high level of intellectual curiosity in their most progressive stages.

Neurobiological basis

The temporal lobe is involved in understanding.[10] Intellectual curiosity might be regarded as the trait that motivates growth of understanding in the temporal lobe. Motivation is effectuated by the neurotransmitter dopamine[11]

Similarity to other concepts

Due to a high level of correlation (.78), it has been argued that need for cognition and typical intellectual engagement basically are measuring the same trait.[12] Intellectual curiosity might be regarded as an umbrella term for these two traits.

References

  1. Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO personality Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  2. Rackham, Marcus Tullius Cicero; with an English translation by H. (2006). De finibus bonorum et malorum (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. p. 48. ISBN 9780674990449.
  3. Selby-Bigge, David Hume. Ed. by L. A. (1987). A treatise of human nature (2nd ed.). Oxford: Clarendon. ISBN 978-0-19-824588-9.
  4. Berlyne, D. E (1954). "A Theory of Human Curiosity". British Journal of Psychology. General Section. 45 (3): 180–91. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1954.tb01243.x. PMID 13190171.
  5. Goff, Maynard; Ackerman, Phillip L (1992). "Personality-intelligence relations: Assessment of typical intellectual engagement". Journal of Educational Psychology. 84 (4): 537–52. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.537.
  6. Engel, Susan. The Hungry Mind: The Origins of Curiosity in Childhood. Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-73675-7.
  7. Swanson, Kristen; Krause, Dianne; Selak, Bill; Casas, Jimmy; Hasty, Desmond; Juliani, A. J.; Gabriele, Anthony; Mogg, Steve; Dougherty, Kevin; Couros, George. Intellectual Curiosity In Our Schools. LeanPub.
  8. von Stumm, Sophie; Hell, Benedikt; Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (2011). "The Hungry Mind". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 6 (6): 574–88. doi:10.1177/1745691611421204. PMID 26168378.
  9. Huff, Toby E. (2010). Intellectual curiosity and the scientific revolution a global perspective (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-17052-9.
  10. Smith; Kosslyn (2007). Cognitive Psychology: Mind and Brain. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. pp. 21, 194–199, 349.
  11. Costa, Vincent D; Tran, Valery L; Turchi, Janita; Averbeck, Bruno B (2014). "Dopamine modulates novelty seeking behavior during decision making". Behavioral Neuroscience. 128 (5): 556–66. doi:10.1037/a0037128. PMC 5861725. PMID 24911320.
  12. Woo, Sang Eun; Harms, Peter D; Kuncel, Nathan R (2007). "Integrating personality and intelligence: Typical intellectual engagement and need for cognition". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (6): 1635–9. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.022.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.