Interpersonal attraction

Interpersonal attraction is the attraction between people which leads to the development of platonic or romantic relationships. It is distinct from perceptions such as physical attractiveness, and involves views of what is and what is not considered beautiful or attractive.

The study of interpersonal attraction is a major area of research in social psychology. Interpersonal attraction is related to how much one likes, dislikes, or hates someone. It can be viewed as a force acting between two people that tends to draw them together and to resist their separation. When measuring interpersonal attraction, one must refer to the qualities of the attracted and those of the attractor to achieve predictive accuracy. It is suggested that to determine attraction, both the personalities and the situation must be taken into account.

Measurement

In social psychology, interpersonal attraction is most-frequently measured using the Interpersonal Attraction Judgment Scale developed by Donn Byrne.[1] It is a scale in which a subject rates another person on factors such as intelligence, knowledge of current events, morality, adjustment, likability, and desirability as a work partner. This scale seems to be directly related with other measures of social attraction such as social choice, feelings of desire for a date, sexual partner or spouse, voluntary physical proximity, frequency of eye contact, etc.

Kiesler and Goldberg analyzed a variety of response measures that were typically utilized as measures of attraction and extracted two factors: the first, characterized as primarily socioemotional, included variables such as liking, desirability of the person's inclusion in social clubs and parties, seating choices, and lunching together. The second factor included variables such as voting for, admiration and respect for, and also seeking the opinion of the target.[1] Another widely used measurement technique scales verbal responses expressed as subjective ratings or judgments of the person of interest.[1]

Causes

There are many factors that lead to interpersonal attraction. Studies suggest that all factors involve social reinforcement.[2] The most frequently studied include physical attractiveness, propinquity (frequency of interaction), familiarity, similarity, complementarity, reciprocal liking, and reinforcement. The impact of familiarity, for example, is shown in the way physical proximity and interaction enhances cohesiveness, a social concept that facilitates communication and positive attitude towards a particular individual on account of similarities or the ability to satisfy important goals.[3] Similarity is believed to more likely lead to liking and attraction rather than differences.[4] Numerous studies have focused on the role of physical attractiveness to personal attraction. One finding was that people tend to attribute positive qualities such as intelligence, competence, and warmth to individuals who have a pleasing physical appearance.[5]

Propinquity effect

The propinquity effect relies on the observation that: "The more we see and interact with a person, the more likely he or she is to become our friend or sexual partner." This effect is very similar to the mere exposure effect in that the more a person is exposed to a stimulus, the more the person likes it; however, there are exceptions.[6] Familiarity can also occur without physical exposure. Recent studies show that relationships formed over the Internet resemble those developed face-to-face, in terms of perceived quality and depth.[7]

Exposure effect

The exposure effect, also known as the familiarity principle, states that the more a person is exposed to something, the more they come to like it. This applies equally to both objects and people.[6] A clear illustration is in a 1992 study: the researchers had four women of similar appearance attend a large college course over a semester such that each woman attended a different number of sessions (0, 5, 10, or 15). Students then rated the women for perceived familiarity, attractiveness and similarity at the end of the term. Results indicated a strong effect of exposure on attraction that was mediated by the effect of exposure on familiarity.[8] However, exposure does not always increase attraction. For example, the social allergy effect can occur when a person grows increasingly annoyed by and hypersensitive to another's repeated behaviors instead of growing more fond of his or her idiosyncrasies over time.[9]

Similarity attraction effect

General

The proverb "birds of a feather flock together" has been used to illustrate that similarity is a crucial determinant of interpersonal attraction.[10] Studies about attraction indicate that people are strongly attracted to lookalikes in physical and social appearance. This similarity is in the broadest sense: similarity in bone-structure, characteristics, life goals and physical appearance. The more these points match, the happier people are in a relationship.[11]

The lookalike effect plays the role of self-affirmation. A person typically enjoys receiving confirmation of aspects of his or her life, ideas, attitudes and personal characteristics, and people seem to look for an image of themselves to spend their life with. A basic principle of interpersonal attraction is the rule of similarity: similarity is attractive — an underlying principle that applies to both friendships and romantic relationships. The proportion of attitudes shared correlates well with the degree of interpersonal attraction. Cheerful people like to be around other cheerful people and negative people would rather be around other negative people.[12] A 2004 study, based on indirect evidence, concluded that humans choose mates based partly on facial resemblance to themselves.[13]

According to Morry's attraction-similarity model (2007), there is a lay belief that people with actual similarity produce initial attraction.[14] The perceived similarity is either self-serving, as in a friendship, or relationship-serving, as in a romantic relationship. In a 1963 study, Theodore Newcomb pointed out that people tend to change perceived similarity to obtain balance in a relationship.[15] Additionally, perceived but not actual similarity was found to predict interpersonal attraction during a face-to-face initial romantic encounter.[16]

In a 1988 study, Lydon, Jamieson & Zanna suggest that interpersonal similarity and attraction are multidimensional constructs in which people are attracted to people similar to themselves in demographics, physical appearance, attitudes, interpersonal style, social and cultural background, personality, preferred interests and activities, and communication and social skills.[17] Newcomb's earlier 1961 study on college-dorm roommates also suggested that individuals with shared backgrounds, academic achievements, attitudes, values, and political views typically became friends.

Physical appearance

The matching hypothesis proposed by sociologist Erving Goffman suggests that people are more likely to form long standing relationships with those who are equally matched in social attributes, like physical attractiveness.[18] The study by researchers Walster and Walster supported the matching hypothesis by showing that partners who were similar in terms of physical attractiveness expressed the most liking for each other.[19] Another study also found evidence that supported the matching hypothesis: photos of dating and engaged couples were rated in terms of attractiveness, and a definite tendency was found for couples of similar attractiveness to date or engage.[20] Several studies support this evidence of similar facial attractiveness. Penton-Voak, Perrett and Peirce (1999) found that subjects rated the pictures with their own face morphed into it as more attractive.[21] DeBruine (2002) demonstrated in her research how subjects entrusted more money to their opponents in a game play, when the opponents were presented as similar to them.[22] Little, Burt & Perrett (2006) examined similarity in sight for married couples and found that the couples were assessed at the same age and level of attractiveness.[23]

A speed-dating experiment done on graduate students from Columbia University showed that although physical attractiveness is preferred in a potential partner, men show a greater preference for it than women.[24] However, more recent work suggests that sex differences in stated ideal partner-preferences for physical attractiveness disappear when examining actual preferences for real-life potential partners.[25] For example, Eastwick and Finkel (2008) failed to find sex differences in the association between initial ratings of physical attractiveness and romantic interest in potential partners during a speed dating paradigm.[26]

Quality of voice

In addition to physical looks, quality of voice has also been shown to enhance interpersonal attraction. Oguchi and Kikuchi (1997) had 25 female students from one university rank the level of vocal attraction, physical attraction, and overall interpersonal attraction of 4 male students from another university. Vocal and physical attractiveness had independent effects on overall interpersonal attraction. In a second part of the same study, these results were replicated in a larger sample of students for both genders (62 subjects, 20 males and 42 females with 16 target students, 8 males and 8 females).[27] Similarly, Zuckerman, Miyake and Hodgins (1991) found that both vocal and physical attractiveness contributed significantly to observers' ratings of targets for general attractiveness.[28] These results suggest that when people evaluate one's voice as attractive, they also tend to evaluate that person as attractive.

Attitudes

According to the law of attraction by Byrne (1971),[29][30] attraction towards a person is positively related to the proportion of 'attitude similarity' associated with that person. Based on the cognitive consistency theories, difference in attitudes and interests can lead to dislike and avoidance whereas similarity in attitudes promotes social attraction.[31][32] Miller (1972) pointed out that attitude similarity activates the perceived attractiveness and favorability information from each other, whereas dissimilarity would reduce the impact of these cues.[33]

The studies by Jamieson, Lydon and Zanna (1987–88) showed that attitude similarity could predict how people evaluate their respect for each other, and also predict social and intellectual first impressions – the former by activity preference similarity and the latter by value-based attitude similarity.[34] In intergroup comparisons, high attitude-similarity would lead to homogeneity among in-group members whereas low attitude-similarity would lead to diversity among in-group members, promoting social attraction and achieving high group performance in different tasks.[35]

Although attitude similarity and attraction are linearly related, attraction may not contribute significantly to attitude change.[36]

Other social and cultural aspects

Byrne, Clore and Worchel (1966) suggested that people with similar economic status are likely to be attracted to each other.[37] Buss & Barnes (1986) also found that people prefer their romantic partners to be similar in certain demographic characteristics, including religious background, political orientation and socio-economic status.[38]

Researchers have shown that interpersonal attraction was positively correlated to personality similarity.[39] People are inclined to desire romantic partners who are similar to themselves on agreeableness, conscientiousness, extroversion, emotional stability, openness to experience,[40] and attachment style.[41]

Activity similarity was especially predictive of liking judgments, which affects the judgments of attraction.[17] According to the post-conversation measures of social attraction, tactical similarity was positively correlated with partner satisfaction and global competence ratings, but was uncorrelated with the opinion change and perceived persuasiveness measures.[42]

When checking similar variables they were also seen as more similar on a number of personality characteristics. This study found that the length of the average relationship was related to perceptions of similarity; the couples who were together longer were seen as more equal. This effect can be attributed to the fact that when time passes by couples become more alike through shared experiences, or that couples that are alike stay together longer.[43]

Similarity has effects on starting a relationship by initial attraction to know each other. It is shown that high attitude similarity resulted in a significant increase in initial attraction to the target person and high attitude dissimilarity resulted in a decrease of initial attraction.[44][45] Similarity also promotes relationship commitment. Study on heterosexual dating couples found that similarity in intrinsic values of the couple was linked to relationship commitment and stability.[46]

Social homogamy refers to "passive, indirect effects on spousal similarity". The result showed that age and education level are crucial in affecting the mate preference. Because people with similar age study and interact more in the same form of the school, propinquity effect (i.e., the tendency of people to meet and spend time with those who share the common characteristics) plays a significant impact in spousal similarity. Convergence refers to an increasing similarity with time. Although the previous research showed that there is a greater effect on attitude and value than on personality traits, however, it is found that initial assortment (i.e., similarity within couples at the beginning of marriage) rather than convergence, plays a crucial role in explaining spousal similarity.[47]

Active assortment refers to direct effects on choosing someone similar to oneself in mating preferences. The data showed that there is a greater effect on political and religious attitudes than on personality traits. A follow-up issue on the reason of the finding was raised. The concepts of idiosyncratic (i.e. different individuals have different mate preferences) and consensual (i.e. a consensus of preference on some prospective mates to others) in mate preference. The data showed that mate preference on political and religious bases tend to be idiosyncratic, for example, a Catholic would be more likely to choose a mate who is also a Catholic, as opposed to a Buddhist. Such idiosyncratic preferences produce a high level of active assortment which plays a vital role in affecting spousal similarity. In summary, active assortment plays a large role, whereas convergence has little evidence on showing such effect.

Complementarity

The model of complementarity explains whether "birds of a feather flock together" or "opposites attract".

Studies show that complementary interaction between two partners increases their attractiveness to each other. Complementary partners preferred closer interpersonal relationship.[48] Couples who reported the highest level of loving and harmonious relationship were more dissimilar in dominance than couples who scored lower in relationship quality.[49]

Mathes and Moore (1985) found that people were more attracted to peers approximating to their ideal self than to those who did not. Specifically, low self-esteem individuals appeared more likely to desire a complementary relationship than high self-esteem people.[50] We are attracted to people who complement us because this allows us to maintain our preferred style of behavior,[49] and interaction with someone who complements our own behavior likely confers a sense of self-validation and security.[51]

Similarity or complementarity

Principles of similarity and complementarity seem to be contradictory on the surface.[52][53] In fact, they agree on the dimension of warmth. Both principles state that friendly people would prefer friendly partners.[54]

The importance of similarity and complementarity may depend on the stage of the relationship. Similarity seems to carry considerable weight in initial attraction, while complementarity assumes importance as the relationship develops over time.[55] Markey (2007) found that people would be more satisfied with their relationship if their partners differed from them, at least in terms of dominance, as two dominant persons may experience conflicts while two submissive individuals may have frustration as neither take the initiative.[49]

Perception and actual behavior might not be congruent with each other. There were cases that dominant people perceived their partners to be similarly dominant, yet to independent observers, the actual behavior of their partner was submissive, i.e. complementary to them.[54] Why people perceive their romantic partners to be similar to them despite evidence of the contrary remains unclear.

Evolutionary theories

The evolutionary theory of human interpersonal attraction states that opposite-sex attraction most often occurs when someone has physical features indicating that he or she is very fertile. Considering that one primary purpose of conjugal/romantic relationships is reproduction, it would follow that people invest in partners who appear very fertile, increasing the chance of their genes being passed down to the next generation. This theory has been criticized because it does not explain relationships between same-sex couples or couples who do not want children, although this may have something to do with the fact that whether one wants children or not one is still subject to the evolutionary forces which produce them.

Evolutionary theory also suggests that people whose physical features suggest they are healthy are seen as more attractive. The theory suggests that a healthy mate is more likely to possess genetic traits related to health that would be passed on to offspring. People's tendency to consider people with facial symmetry more attractive than those with less symmetrical faces is one example. However, a test was conducted that found that perfectly symmetrical faces were less attractive than normal faces. According to this study, the exact ratio of symmetric to asymmetric facial features depicting the highest attraction is still undetermined.[56]

It has also been suggested that people are attracted to faces similar to their own as these features serve as cues of kinship. This preference for facial-resemblance is thought to vary across contexts. For example, a study by DeBruine et al. (2008) found that individuals rated faces which had been manipulated to be similar to their own as having more prosocial attributes, but were less likely to find them sexually attractive. These results support "inclusive fitness theory", which predicts that organisms will help closely related kin over more distant relatives. Results further suggest inherent mate-selective mechanisms that consider costs of inbreeding to offspring health.[57]

Increased female attraction to men in relationships

A 2009 study by Melissa Burkley and Jessica Parker found that 59% of women tested were interested in pursuing a relationship with an "ideal" single man (who was, unknown to the women, fictitious).[58] When they believed the "ideal" man was already in a romantic relationship, 90% of the women were interested in a romantic relationship.

Breaking up

There are several reasons that a relationship, whether friendly or romantic, may come to an end (break up). One reason derives from the equity theory: if a person in the relationship feels that the personal costs of being in the relationship outweigh the rewards there is a strong chance that this person will end the relationship. For instance, the costs may outweigh the rewards due to guilt and shame.

See also

Notes

  1. 1 2 3 Byrne, Donn; Griffitt, William (1973). "Interpersonal Attraction". Annual Review of Psychology. 24: 316–336. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.24.020173.001533.
  2. Carlson, N. R. (1999–2000). Psychology: The Science of Behaviour (Canadian ed.). Scarborough, Ontario: Allyn and Bacon Canada. pp. 506–507.
  3. Reis, Harry; Sprecher, Susan (2009). Encyclopedia of Human Relationships. London: SAGE Publications, Inc. ISBN 978-1-4129-5846-2.
  4. Breckler, Steven; Olson, James; Wiggins, Elizabeth (2005). Social Psychology Alive. Belmont, CA: Thomas Wadsworth. p. 525. ISBN 0-534-57834-9.
  5. Weiten, Wayne (2014). Psychology: Themes and Variations, Briefer Version. Cengage Learning. p. 432. ISBN 1-133-95783-8.
  6. 1 2 Miller, R.; Perlman, D.; Brehm, S. (2006). Intimate Relationships. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  7. Bargh, J. A.; McKenna, K. Y. A. (2004). "The internet and social life". Annual Review of Psychology. 55: 573–590. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141922.
  8. Moreland, Richard L.; Beach, Scott R. (May 1992). "Exposure effects in the classroom: The development of affinity among students". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 28: 255–276. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(92)90055-O.
  9. Cunningham, Michael R.; Shamblen, Stephen R.; Barbee, Anita P.; Ault, Lara K. (April 2005). "Social allergies in romantic relationships: Behavioral repetition, emotional sensitization, and dissatisfaction in dating couples". Personal Relationships. 12: 273–295. doi:10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00115.x.
  10. Heine, Steven J; Foster, Julie-Ann B.; Spina, Roy (2009). "Do birds of a feather universally flock together? Cultural variation in the similarity-attraction effect". Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 12: 247–258. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2009.01289.x.
  11. Folkes, V. S. (1982). "Forming relationships and the matching hypothesis". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 8: 631–636. doi:10.1177/0146167282084005.
  12. Locke, Kenneth D.; Horowitz, Leonard M. (1990). "Satisfaction in interpersonal interactions as a function of similarity in level of dysphoria" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (5): 823–831. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.823. ISSN 1939-1315.
  13. Alvarez, Liliana; Jaffe, Klaus (2004). "Narcissism guides mate selection: Humans mate assortatively, as revealed by facial resemblance, following an algorithm of "self seeking like"" (PDF). Evolutionary Psychology. 2: 177–194. doi:10.1177/147470490400200123.
  14. Morry, M. M. (2007). "Relationship satisfaction as a predictor of perceived similarity among cross-sex friends: A test of the attraction-similarity model". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 24: 117–138. doi:10.1177/0265407507072615.
  15. Newcomb, T. M. (1963). "Stabilities underlying changes in interpersonal attraction". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 (4): 376–386. doi:10.1037/h0041059.
  16. Tidwell, Natasha; Eastwick, Paul; Finkel, Eli (June 2013). "Perceived, not actual, similarity predicts initial attraction in a live romantic context: Evidence from the speed-dating paradigm" (PDF). Personal Relationships. 20 (2): 199–215. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2012.01405.x.
  17. 1 2 Lydon, J. E.; Jamieson, D. W.; Zanna, M. P. (1988). "Interpersonal similarity and the social and intellectual dimensions of first impressions". Social Cognition. 6 (4): 269–286. doi:10.1521/soco.1988.6.4.269.
  18. Berkowitz, Leonard (1974). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Volume 7. pp. 159–160. ISBN 0-12-015207-X.
  19. Berscheid, Ellen; Dion, Karen; Walster, Elaine; Walster, G. William (1 March 1971). "Physical attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the matching hypothesis" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 7 (2): 173–189. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(71)90065-5. ISSN 0022-1031.
  20. Murstein, Bernard I.; Christy, Patricia (October 1976). "Physical attractiveness and marriage adjustment in middle-aged couples". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 34 (4): 537–542. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.4.537.
  21. Voak, I. S. Penton-; Perrett, D. I.; Peirce, J. W. (1 March 1999). "Computer graphic studies of the role of facial similarity in judgements of attractiveness" (PDF). Current Psychology. 18 (1): 104–117. doi:10.1007/s12144-999-1020-4. ISSN 0737-8262.
  22. DeBruine, L. M. (7 July 2002). "Facial resemblance enhances trust" (PDF). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 269 (1498): 1307–1312. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2034. ISSN 0962-8452.
  23. Little, Anthony C.; Burt, D. Michael; Perrett, David I. (1 April 2006). "Assortative mating for perceived facial personality traits" (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 40 (5): 973–984. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.016. ISSN 0191-8869.
  24. Fisman, Raymond; Iyengar, Sheena S; Kamenica, Emir; Simonson, Itamar (28 April 2006). "Gender Differences in Mate Selection: Evidence from a Speed Dating Experiment" (PDF). Quarterly Journal of Economics. 121 (2): 673–697. doi:10.1162/qjec.2006.121.2.673.
  25. Luo, Shanhong; Zhang, Guangjian (August 2009). "What Leads to Romantic Attraction: Similarity, Reciprocity, Security, or Beauty? Evidence From a Speed-Dating Study". Journal of Personality. 77 (4): 933–964. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00570.x.
  26. Eastwick, Paul; Finkel, Eli J. (February 2008). "Sex Differences in Mate Preferences Revisited: Do People Know What They Initially Desire in a Romantic Partner?" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 94 (2): 245–264. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.245. PMID 18211175.
  27. Oguchi, Takashi; Kikuchi, Hiroto (March 1997). "Voice and Interpersonal Attraction". Japanese Psychological Research. 39 (2): 56–61. doi:10.1111/1468-5884.00037.
  28. Zuckerman, Miron; Miyake, Kunitate; Hodgins, Holley S. (April 1991). "Cross-channel effects of vocal and physical attractiveness and their implications for interpersonal perception". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (4): 545–554. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.4.545.
  29. Not to be confused with the 'law of attraction' discussed by a different Byrne, the metaphysical writer Rhonda Byrne.
  30. Byrne, Donn Erwin (1971). The attraction paradigm. Academic Press.
  31. Singh, R.; Ho, S. Y. (2000). "Attitudes and attraction: A new test of the attraction, repulsion and similarity-dissimilarity asymmetry hypotheses". British Journal of Social Psychology. 39 (2): 197–211. doi:10.1348/014466600164426.
  32. Byrne, D.; London, O.; Reeves, K. (1968). "The effects of physical attractiveness, sex, and attitude similarity on interpersonal attraction". Journal of Personality. 36 (2): 259–271. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1968.tb01473.x. PMID 5660731.
  33. Miller, A. G. (1972). "Effect of attitude similarity-dissimilarity on the utilization of additional stimulus inputs in judgments of interpersonal attraction". Psychonomic Science. 26 (4): 199–203. doi:10.3758/bf03328593.
  34. Jamieson, D. W. Lydon; Zanna, M. P.; Zanna, Mark P. (1987). "Attitude and activity preference similarity: Differential bases of interpersonal attraction for low and high self-monitors". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 53 (6): 1052–1060. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1052.
  35. Hahn, D.; Hwang, S. (1999). "Test of similarity-attraction hypothesis in group performance situation". Korean Journal of Social & Personality Psychology. 13 (1): 255–275.
  36. Simons, H. W.; Berkowitz, N. N.; Moyer, R. J. (1970). "Similarity, credibility, and attitude change: A review and a theory". Psychological Bulletin. 73 (1): 1–16. doi:10.1037/h0028429.
  37. Byrne, D.; Clore, G. L. J.; Worchel, P. (1966). "Effect of economic similarity-dissimilarity on interpersonal attraction". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 4 (2): 220–224. doi:10.1037/h0023559.
    • Buss, D. M.; Barnes, M. (1986). "Preferences in human mate selection" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 50 (3): 559–570. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.559.
  38. Goldman, J. A.; Rosenzweig, C. M.; Lutter, A. D. (1980). "Effect of similarity of ego identity status on interpersonal attraction". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 9 (2): 153–162. doi:10.1007/BF02087933.
  39. Botwin, M. D.; Buss, D. M.; Shackelford, T. K. (1997). "Personality and mate preferences: Five factors in mate selection and marital satisfaction" (PDF). Journal of Personality. 65 (1): 107–136. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00531.x. PMID 9143146.
  40. Klohnen, E. C.; Luo, S. (2003). "Interpersonal attraction and personality: What is attractive – self similarity, ideal similarity, complementarity, or attachment security?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 85 (4): 709–722. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.709. PMID 14561124.
  41. Waldron, V. R.; Applegate, J. L. (1998). "Similarity in the use of person-centered tactics: Effects on social attraction and persuasiveness in dyadic verbal disagreements". Communication Reports. 11 (2): 155–165. doi:10.1080/08934219809367697.
  42. Zajonc, R. B.; Adelmann, Pamela K.; Murphy, Sheila T.; Niedenthal, Paula M. (1 December 1987). "Convergence in the physical appearance of spouses" (PDF). Motivation and Emotion. 11 (4): 335–346. doi:10.1007/BF00992848. ISSN 0146-7239.
  43. Gutkin, T. B.; Gridley, G. C.; Wendt, J. M. (1976). "The effect of initial attraction and attitude similarity-dissimilarity on interpersonal attraction". Cornell Journal of Social Relations. 11 (2): 153–160.
  44. Kaplan, M. F.; Olczak, P. V. (1971). "Attraction toward another as a function of similarity and commonality of attitudes". Psychological Reports. 28 (2): 515–521. doi:10.2466/pr0.1971.28.2.515.
  45. Kurdek, L. A.; Schnopp-Wyatt, D. (1997). "Predicting relationship commitment and relationship stability from both partners' relationship values: Evidence from heterosexual dating couples". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 23 (10): 1111–1119. doi:10.1177/01461672972310011.
  46. Watson, D.; Klohnen, E. C.; Casillas, A.; Nus, S. E.; Haig, J.; Berry, D. S. (2004). "Match makers and deal breakers: Analyses of assortative mating in newlywed couples". Journal of Personality. 72 (5): 1034. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00289.x. PMID 15335336.
  47. Nowicki, S. Jr.; Manheim, S. (1991). "Interpersonal complementarity and time of interaction in female relationships". Journal of Research in Personality. 25 (3): 322–333. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(91)90023-J.
  48. 1 2 3 Markey, P.M.; Markey, C. N. (2007). "Romantic ideals, romantic obtainment, and relationship experiences: The complementarity of interpersonal traits among romantic partners" (PDF). Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 24 (4): 517–533. doi:10.1177/0265407507079241.
  49. Mathes, E. W.; Moore, C. L. (1985). "Reik's complementarily theory of romantic love". The Journal of Social Psychology. 125 (3): 321–327. doi:10.1080/00224545.1985.9922893.
  50. Carson, Robert C. (1969). Interaction concepts of personality. Aldine Pub. Co.
  51. Posavac, E. J. (1971). "Dimensions of trait preferences and personality type". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 19 (3): 274–281. doi:10.1037/h0031467.
  52. Klohnen, E. C.; Mendelsohn, G. A. (1998). "Partner Selection for Personality Characteristics: A Couple-Centered Approach". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 24 (3): 268–278. doi:10.1177/0146167298243004.
  53. 1 2 M. Horowitz, Leonard; Dryer, Christopher; N. Krasnoperova, Elena (1 January 1997). "The circumplex structure of interpersonal problems". doi:10.1037/10261-015.
  54. Vinacke, W. E.; Shannon, K.; Palazzo, V; Balsavage, L. (1988). "Similarity and complementarity in intimate couples". Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 114: 51–76.
  55. Swaddle, John P.; Cuthill, Innes C. (1995). "Asymmetry and Human Facial Attractiveness: Symmetry May not Always be Beautiful". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. Royal Society. 261 (1360): 111–16. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0124. ISSN 0962-8452. JSTOR 50054.
  56. DeBruine, Lisa M.; Jones, Benedict C.; Little, Anthony C.; Perrett, David I. (2008). "Social Perception of Facial Resemblance in Humans". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 37: 64–77. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9266-0.
  57. Coghlan, Andy (17 August 2009). "It's true: all the taken men are best". New Scientist.

References

  • Aronson, Elliot; Wilson, Timothy D.; Akert, Robin M. (2007). Social Psychology (6th ed.). New Jersey: Upper Saddle River.
  • Drayer, D. C.; Horowitz, Leonard M. (1997). "When do opposites attract? Interpersonal complementarity versus similarity" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (3): 592–603. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.592.
  • Kubitschek, Warren N.; Hallinan, Maureen T. (1998). "Tracking and Students' Friendships". Social Psychology Quarterly. 61 (1): 1–15. doi:10.2307/2787054. ISSN 0190-2725. JSTOR 2787054.
  • Johnson, Claudia (2010). Names and Your Future. Random Publishing House. p. 279.
  • Montoya, R. Matthew; Horton, Robert S. (2004). "On the Importance of Cognitive Evaluation as a Determinant of Interpersonal Attraction" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (5): 696–712. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.696. ISSN 1939-1315.
  • Moskowitz; Ho, Moon-ho Ringo; Turcotte-tremblay, Anne-marie (2007). "Contextual Influences on Interpersonal Complementarity" (PDF). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 33 (8): 1051–1063. doi:10.1177/0146167207303024.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.